Nilo-Saharan languages

The Nilo-Saharan languages are a proposed family of African languages spoken by some 50–60 million people, mainly in the upper parts of the Chari and Nile rivers, including historic Nubia, north of where the two tributaries of the Nile meet. The languages extend through 17 nations in the northern half of Africa: from Algeria to Benin in the west; from Libya to the Democratic Republic of the Congo in the centre; and from Egypt to Tanzania in the east.

Nilo-Saharan
(disputed)
Geographic
distribution
Central Africa, north-central Africa and East Africa
Linguistic classificationIf valid, one of the world's primary language families
Subdivisions
ISO 639-2 / 5ssa
GlottologNone
Map showing the distribution of Nilo-Saharan languages

As indicated by its hyphenated name, Nilo-Saharan is a family of the African interior, including the greater Nile Basin and the Central Sahara Desert. Eight of its proposed constituent divisions (excluding Kunama, Kuliak, and Songhay) are found in the modern two nations of Sudan and South Sudan, through which the Nile River flows.

In his book The Languages of Africa (1963), Joseph Greenberg named the group and argued it was a genetic family. It contains the languages which are not included in the Niger–Congo, Afroasiatic or Khoisan groups. Although some linguists have referred to the phylum as "Greenberg's wastebasket," into which he placed all the otherwise unaffiliated non-click languages of Africa, [1][2] specialists in the field have accepted its reality since Greenberg's classification.[3] Its supporters accept that it is a challenging proposal to demonstrate but contend that it looks more promising the more work is done.[4][5][6]

Some of the constituent groups of Nilo-Saharan are estimated to predate the African neolithic. Thus, the unity of Eastern Sudanic is estimated to date to at least the 5th millennium BC.[7] Nilo-Saharan genetic unity would necessarily be much older still and date to the late Upper Paleolithic.

This larger classification system is not accepted by all linguists, however. Glottolog (2013), for example, a publication of the Max Planck Institute in Germany, does not recognise the unity of the Nilo-Saharan family or even of the Eastern Sudanic branch; Georgiy Starostin (2016) likewise does not accept a relationship between the branches of Nilo-Saharan, though he leaves open the possibility that some of them may prove to be related to each other once the necessary reconstructive work is done.

Characteristics

The constituent families of Nilo-Saharan are quite diverse. One characteristic feature is a tripartite singulativecollectiveplurative number system, which Blench (2010) believes is a result of a noun-classifier system in the protolanguage. The distribution of the families may reflect ancient water courses in a green Sahara during the Neolithic Subpluvial, when the desert was more habitable than it is today.[8]

Major languages

Within the Nilo-Saharan languages are a number of languages with at least a million speakers (most data from SIL's Ethnologue 16 (2009)). In descending order:

Some other important Nilo-Saharan languages under 1 million speakers:

The total for all speakers of Nilo-Saharan languages according to Ethnologue 16 is 38–39 million people. However, the data spans a range from ca. 1980 to 2005, with a weighted median at ca. 1990. Given population growth rates, the figure in 2010 might be half again higher, or about 60 million.

History of the proposal

The Saharan family (which includes Kanuri, Kanembu, the Tebu languages, and Zaghawa) was recognized by Heinrich Barth in 1853, the Nilotic languages by Karl Richard Lepsius in 1880, the various constituent branches of Central Sudanic (but not the connection between them) by Friedrich Müller in 1889, and the Maban family by Maurice Gaudefroy-Demombynes in 1907. The first inklings of a wider family came in 1912, when Diedrich Westermann included three of the (still independent) Central Sudanic families within Nilotic in a proposal he called Niloto-Sudanic;[10] this expanded Nilotic was in turn linked to Nubian, Kunama, and possibly Berta, essentially Greenberg's Macro-Sudanic (Chari–Nile) proposal of 1954.

In 1920 G. W. Murray fleshed out the Eastern Sudanic languages when he grouped Nilotic, Nubian, Nera, Gaam, and Kunama. Carlo Conti Rossini made similar proposals in 1926, and in 1935 Westermann added Murle. In 1940 A. N. Tucker published evidence linking five of the six branches of Central Sudanic alongside his more explicit proposal for East Sudanic. In 1950 Greenberg retained Eastern Sudanic and Central Sudanic as separate families, but accepted Westermann's conclusions of four decades earlier in 1954 when he linked them together as Macro-Sudanic (later Chari–Nile, from the Chari and Nile Watersheds).

Greenberg's later contribution came in 1963, when he tied Chari–Nile to Songhai, Saharan, Maban, Fur, and Koman-Gumuz and coined the current name Nilo-Saharan for the resulting family. Lionel Bender noted that Chari–Nile was a historical artifact of the discovery of the family and did not reflect an exclusive relationship between these languages, and the group has been abandoned, with its constituents becoming primary branches of Nilo-Saharan—or, equivalently, Chari–Nile and Nilo-Saharan have merged, with the name Nilo-Saharan retained. When it was realized that the Kadu languages were not Niger–Congo, they were commonly assumed to therefore be Nilo-Saharan, but this remains somewhat controversial.

Progress has been made since Greenberg established the plausibility of the family. Koman and Gumuz remain poorly attested and are difficult to work with, while arguments continue over the inclusion of Songhai. Blench (2010) believes that the distribution of Nilo-Saharan reflects the waterways of the wet Sahara 12,000 years ago, and that the protolanguage had noun classifiers, which today are reflected in a diverse range of prefixes, suffixes, and number marking.

Internal relationships

Dimmendaal (2008) notes that Greenberg (1963) based his conclusion on strong evidence and that the proposal as a whole has become more convincing in the decades since. Mikkola (1999) reviewed Greenberg's evidence and found it convincing. Roger Blench notes morphological similarities in all putative branches, which leads him to believe that the family is likely to be valid.

Koman and Gumuz are poorly known and have been difficult to evaluate until recently. Songhay is markedly divergent, in part due to massive influence from the Mande languages [3]. Also problematic are the Kuliak languages, which are spoken by hunter-gatherers and appear to retain a non-Nilo-Saharan core; Blench believes they may have been similar to Hadza or Dahalo and shifted incompletely to Nilo-Saharan.

Anbessa Tefera and Peter Unseth consider the poorly attested Shabo language to be Nilo-Saharan, though unclassified within the family due to lack of data; Dimmendaal and Blench consider it to be a language isolate on current evidence. Proposals have sometimes been made to add Mande (usually included in Niger–Congo), largely due to its many noteworthy similarities with Songhay rather than with Nilo-Saharan as a whole, however this relationship is more likely due to a close relationship between Songhay and Mande many thousands of years ago in the early days of Nilo-Saharan, so the relationship is probably more one of ancient contact than a genetic link [3].

The extinct Meroitic language of ancient Kush has been accepted by linguists such as Rille, Dimmendaal, and Blench as Nilo-Saharan, though others argue for an Afroasiatic affiliation. It is poorly attested.

There is little doubt that the constituent families of Nilo-Saharan—of which only Eastern Sudanic and Central Sudanic show much internal diversity—are valid groups. However, there have been several conflicting classifications in grouping them together. Each of the proposed higher-order groups has been rejected by other researchers: Greenberg's Chari–Nile by Bender and Blench, and Bender's Core Nilo-Saharan by Dimmendaal and Blench. What remains are eight (Dimmendaal) to twelve (Bender) constituent families of no consensus arrangement.

Greenberg 1963

The branches of the Nilo-Saharan languages.

Joseph Greenberg, in The Languages of Africa, set up the family with the following branches. The Chari–Nile core are the connections that had been suggested by previous researchers.

 NiloSaharan 

Koman (including Gumuz)

Saharan

Songhay

Fur

Maban

 Chari–Nile 

Central Sudanic

Kunama

Berta

Eastern Sudanic (including Kuliak, Nubian and Nilotic)

Gumuz was not recognized as distinct from neighboring Koman; it was separated out (forming "Komuz") by Bender (1989).

Bender 1989, 1991

Lionel Bender came up with a classification which expanded upon and revised that of Greenberg. He considered Fur and Maban to constitute a Fur–Maban branch, added Kadu to Nilo-Saharan, removed Kuliak from Eastern Sudanic, removed Gumuz from Koman (but left it as a sister node), and chose to posit Kunama as an independent branch of the family. By 1991 he had added more detail to the tree, dividing Chari–Nile into nested clades, including a Core group in which Berta was considered divergent, and coordinating Fur–Maban as a sister clade to Chari–Nile.[11][12]

 Nilo-Saharan 

Songhay

Saharan

Kunama–Ilit

Kuliak

 Fur–Maban 

Fur

Maban

 Chari–Nile 
 Central Sudanic 

Moru–Mangbetu

Sara–Bongo

 Core 

Berta

 East Sudanic 

SurmicNilotic

Nubian, Nara, Taman

 Komuz 

Gumuz

Koman (including Shabo)

Kadugli–Krongo

Bender revised his model of Nilo-Saharan again in 1996, at which point he split Koman and Gumuz into completely separate branches of Core Nilo-Saharan.[13]

Ehret 1989

Christopher Ehret came up with a novel classification of Nilo-Saharan as a preliminary part of his then-ongoing research into the macrofamily. His evidence for the classification was not fully published until much later (see Ehret 2001 below), and so it did not attain the same level of acclaim as competing proposals, namely those of Bender and Blench.[12]

Bender 2000

By 2000 Bender had entirely abandoned the Chari–Nile and Komuz branches. He also added Kunama back to the "Satellite–Core" group and simplified the subdivisions therein. He retracted the inclusion of Shabo, stating that it could not yet be adequately classified but might prove to be Nilo-Saharan once sufficient research has been done. This tentative and somewhat conservative classification held as a sort of standard for the next decade.[14]

 Nilo-Saharan 

Songhay

Saharan

Kuliak

 Satellite–Core 

Maban

Fur

Central Sudanic

Berta

Kunama

 Core 

Eastern Sudanic

Koman

Gumuz

Kadu

Ehret 2001

Ehret's updated classification was published in his book A Historical–Comparative Reconstruction of Nilo-Saharan (2001).[15] This model is notable in that it consists of two primary branches: Gumuz–Koman, and a Sudanic group containing the rest of the families (see Sudanic languages § Nilo-Saharan for more detail). Also, unusually, Songhay is well-nested within a core group and coordinate with Maban in a "Western Sahelian" clade, and Kadu is not included in Nilo-Saharan. Note that "Koman" in this classification is equivalent to Komuz, i.e. a family with Gumuz and Koman as primary branches, and Ehret renames the traditional Koman group as "Western Koman".

 Nilo-Saharan 
 Koman 

Gumuz

Western Koman

 Sudanic 

Central Sudanic

 Northern Sudanic 

Kunama

 Saharo-Sahelian 

Saharan

 Sahelian 

Fur

Trans-Sahel 
 Western Sahelian 

Songhay

Maban

Eastern Sahelian (Eastern Sudanic) (including Berta)

Blench 2010

With a better understanding of Nilo-Saharan classifiers, and the affixes or number marking they have developed into in various branches, Blench believes that all of the families postulated as Nilo-Saharan belong together. He proposes the following tentative internal classification, with Songhai closest to Saharan, a relationship that had not previously been suggested:

Kunama

Berta

Koman

Gumuz

Saharan

Songhay

Kuliak

Maban

Fur

Kadu

Central Sudanic

Eastern Sudanic

? Mimi of Decorse

Blench 2015

By 2015,[16] and again in 2017,[17] Blench had refined the subclassification of this model, linking Maban with Fur, Kadu with Eastern Sudanic, and Kuliak with the node that contained them, for the following structure:


Berta

Koman

Gumuz

Kunama

Saharan

Songhay

 Central African 

Kuliak

Maban

Fur

Central Sudanic

Kadu

Eastern Sudanic

Starostin (2016)

Starostin's "Macro-Sudanic" in purple, surrounding language families shown as well.

Georgiy Starostin (2016),[18] using lexicostatistics based on Swadesh lists, is more inclusive than Glottolog, and in addition finds probable and possible links between the families that will require reconstruction of the protolanguages for confirmation.

In addition to the families listed in Glottolog (previous section), Starostin considers the following to be established:

A relationship of Nyima with Nubian, Nara, and Tama (NNT) is considered "highly likely" and close enough that proper comparative work should be able to demonstrate the connection if it's valid, though it would fall outside NNT proper (see Eastern Sudanic languages).

Other units that are "highly likely" to eventually prove to be valid families are:

In summary, at this level of certainty, "Nilo-Saharan" constitutes ten distinct and separate language families: Eastern Sudanic, Central Sudanic – Kadu, Maba–Kunama, Komuz, Saharan, Songhai, Kuliak, Fur, Berta, and Shabo.

Possible further "deep" connections, which cannot be evaluated until the proper comparative work on the constituent branches has been completed, are:

  • Eastern Sudanic + Fur + Berta
  • Central Sudanic Kadu + MabaKunama

There are faint suggestions that Eastern and Central Sudanic may be related (essentially the old ChariNile clade), though that possibility is "unexplorable under current conditions" and could be complicated if NigerCongo were added to the comparison. Starostin finds no evidence that the Komuz, Kuliak, Saharan, Songhai, or Shabo languages are related to any of the other Nilo-Saharan languages. Mimi-D and Meroitic were not considered, though Starostin had previously proposed that Mimi-D was also an isolate despite its slight similarity to Central Sudanic.

In a follow up study published in 2017, Starostin reiterated his previous points as well as explicitly accepting a genetic relationship between Macro-East Sudanic and Macro-Central Sudanic. Starostin names this proposal "Macro-Sudanic"[19]

Dimmendaal 2016, 2019

Gerrit J. Dimmendaal[20][21] suggests the following subclassification of Nilo-Saharan:

 NiloSaharan 
Northeastern

Maban

Kunama

Fur

Saharan

Eastern Sudanic (including Berta)

Kuliak

Central Sudanic

Dimmendaal et al. consider the evidence for the inclusion of Kadu and Songhay too weak to draw any conclusions at present, whereas whereas there is some evidence that Koman and Gumuz belong together and may be Nilo-Saharan.[22]

The large Northeastern division is based on several typological markers:

  • tolerance of complex syllable structure
  • higher amount of both inflectional and derivational morphology, including the presence of cases
  • verb-final (SOV or OSV) word order
  • coverb + light verb constructions
  • converbs

Glottolog 4.0 (2019)

In summarizing the literature to date, Hammarström et al. in Glottolog do not accept that the following families are demonstrably related with current research:

External relations

Proposals for the external relationships of Nilo-Saharan typically center on Niger–Congo: Gregersen (1972) grouped the two together as Kongo–Saharan. However, Blench (2011) proposed that the similarities between Niger–Congo and Nilo-Saharan (specifically Atlantic–Congo and Central Sudanic) are due to contact, with the noun-class system of Niger–Congo developed from, or elaborated on the model of, the noun classifiers of Central Sudanic.

Phonology

Nilo-Saharan languages present great differences, being a highly diversified group. It has proven difficult to reconstruct many aspects of Proto-Nilo-Saharan. Two very different reconstructions of the proto-language have been proposed by Lionel Bender and Christopher Ehret.

Bender's reconstruction

The consonant system reconstructed by Bender for Proto-Nilo-Saharan is:

Labial Coronal Palatal Velar
plosive voiceless *t, *t2 *k, *kʰ
voiced *b *d, *d2 *g
fricative *f *s
liquid *r, *l *r2
nasal *m *n
semivowel *w *j

The phonemes /*d2, *t2/ correspond to coronal plosives, the phonetic details are difficult to specify, but clearly, they remain distinct from /*d, *t/ and supported by many phonetic correspondences (another author, C. Ehret, reconstructs for the coronal area the sound [d̪], [ḍ] and [t̪], [ṭ] which perhaps are closer to the phonetic detail of /*d2, *t2/, see infra)

Bender gave a list of about 350 cognates and discussed in depth the grouping and the phonological system proposed by Ch. Ehret. Blench (2000) compares both systems (Bender's and Ehret's) and prefers the former because it is more secure and is based in more reliable data.[23] For example, Bender points out that there is a set of phonemes including implosives /*ɓ, *ɗ, *ʄ, *ɠ/, ejectives /*pʼ, *tʼ, (*sʼ), *cʼ, *kʼ/ and prenasal constants /*mb, *nd, (*nt), *ñɟ, *ŋg/, but it seems that they can be reconstructed only for core groups (E, I, J, L) and the collateral group (C, D, F, G, H), but not for Proto-Nilo-Saharan.

Ehret's reconstruction

Christopher Ehret used a less clear methodology and proposed a maximalist phonemic system:

Labial Dental Alveol. Retrof. Palatal Velar Glottal
plosive implosive *ɗ̣
voiced *b *d̪ *d *ḍ *g
voiceless *p *t̪ *t *ṭ *k
aspirate *pʰ *t̪ʰ *tʰ *ṭʰ *kʰ
ejective *pʼ *t̪̕ *tʼ *ṭ̕ *kʼ
fricative *s, *z *ṣ
nasal simple *m *n
prenasal *ⁿb *ⁿð *ⁿd *ⁿḍ *ⁿg
liquid *l̪ *r, *l
approximant plain *w *j
complex *ʼw *ʼj *h

Ehret's maximalist system has been criticized by Bender and Blench. These authors state that the correspondences used by Ehret are not very clear and because of this many of the sounds in the table may only be allophonic variations.[24]

Morphology

Dimmendaal (2016)[20] cites the following morphological elements as stable across Nilo-Saharan:

Comparative vocabulary

Sample basic vocabulary in different Nilo-Saharan branches:

Note: In table cells with slashes, the singular form is given before the slash, while the plural form follows the slash.

Languageeyeearnosetoothtonguemouthbloodbonetreewatereatname
Proto-Nilotic[25]*(k)ɔŋ, pl. *(k)ɔɲ*yit̪*(q)ume*kɛ-la(-c)*ŋa-lyɛp*(k)ʊt̪ʊk*käw*kɛ-ɛt, *kɪ-yat*pi(-ʀ)*ɲam*ka-ʀin
Proto-Jebel[26]**ed ~ *er**si(di ~ gi)**ɲi-di**kala-d**udu**k-afa-d**(g-)am-**kaca**cii ~ *kii**ɲam(siigə, saag)
Temein[27]nɪ́ŋɪ̀nàʈ / kɛ̀ɛ́nwénàʈ / kwèénkɪ́mɪ́nʈɪ̀n / kɪkɪ́mɪ́nʈɪ́nɪ̀awɪ̀s / kɛ́ɛ̀ʔmɛ́nɖɪnyàʈíʈùk / k(w)úʈɪ̀nmónɪ̀ʈàmɪ̀s / kɔ́maʔmɛ́rɛŋɪ̀s / mɛ́rɛŋmúŋlámakàlɪ́n, kàlɪ́ŋ
Proto-Daju[28]*aŋune / *aŋwe ~ *aŋun*wunute / *wunuge*mu-ne*ɲiɣte / *ɲiɣke*ɲabire / *ɲabirta*ikke / *ikku*tamuke*ŋai / *ŋayu*ewete / *ewe*ma-*si-*ange / *angu
Kadugli (Talla dialect)[29]ayyɛ / iyyɛnaasɔ / isinɛ́ámb-/nigáŋg-árɔkt̪- / iŋŋiniáŋdáɗuk / ni-niinɔ / niginíínɔariid̪ʊt̪iŋguba / kubaffa / nááfaɓiid̪iooriɛɛrɛ / nigirɛɛnɛ
Proto-Northern Eastern Sudanic[30]*maɲ*ɲog-ul*em-u*ŋes-il*ŋal*ag-il ~ *ag-ul*ug-er*kɛs-ɛr*koɲ-er-*mban*kal- / *kamb-*(ŋ)ɛr-i
Nara[30]no, nòò / no-ta, nóó-tatús / túsádemmo, dəmmo, dàm̀mò, dòmmònɪ̀hɪ̀ / nɪ̀hɪ̀t-tá; nèʃɪ̀ / nèʃáhàggà, àggà, ààdà, hàdàaùlò / aùl-lá; àgúrá / àgúr-tàkitto, kɪ̀tòketti, kəti, kátɪ́ / ketta, kátátüm, tûm; kè́lemba, mbààkal, kál, kárade, ààdà
Proto-Nubian[30]*maaɲ, sg. *miɲ-di*ugul(-e), sg. *ugul-di?*ŋil, sg. *ŋíl-di*ŋal, sg. ŋal-di*agil*ùg-er*kiser, sg. *kisir-ti*koor, sg. *koor-ti*es-ti*kal-*er-i
Proto-Taman[30]*me-ti, pl. *mVŋ*(ŋ)usu-ti (sg)*eme, sg. emi-ti (sg.)*ŋesi-t(i), pl. *ŋes-oŋ*laat*auli*agi*kei-ti, pl. *kei-ŋ*gaan; *kiɲe(-ti) (?)*kal /*kaal*ŋan-*(ŋ)aat, pl. *(ŋ)ari-g
Proto-Nyima[30]*a̍ŋV*ɲɔgɔr-*(o)mud̪- (?)*ŋil-?*ŋàl-*wule*amV*t̪uma*bɔ́ŋ*t̪a̍l- / *ta̍m-
Proto-SW Surmic[31]*kɛɓɛrɛ (pl.)*it̪t̪at*ʊŋɛtʃ (?)*ɲiggɪtta*ʌgʌʌt*(k)-ʊt̪t̪ʊk*ɓɪj-*ɛmmɛ*kɛɛt̪*maam*ɗak-*ðara
Proto-SE Surmic[31]*kabari*ɲabi (?)*giroŋ*ɲigidda (?)*kat*tuk-*ɲaɓa*giga (?)*kɛdo (?)*ma*sara
Proto-Kuliak[32]*ekw, pl. *ekw=ẹk*beos, pl. *beosẹk*nyab, pl. *nyabẹk*ɛd-eɓ*ak, pl. *akẹk*seh*ɔk*ad, pl. *ad=is*kywɛh*yed, pl. *yedẹk
Shabo[33]k’itisonɑk’ɑuhɑndɑkɑusɛdɑmoemɑhɑ; egegek’ɔnɑwɔːwoŋgɑse
Ongota[34]ˈʔaːfaˈwoːwaˈsiːna (loan?)ʔitiˈmaʔɑdabo (loan?)ˈʔiːfaˈmitʃa (loan?)ˈhɑntʃaˈtʃaːhawaʔeˈdʒakˈmiʃa
Proto-Sara-Bongo-Bagirmi[35]*kamɔ; *kamu; *kama*imbi; *EmbE; *mbili; *mbElE; *imbil-; *EmbEl-*Samɔ; *Samu; *Somu; *kanu; *kunu; *kVnV*kanga; *nganga*unɖɛ(C-)*tara*manga; *masu; *mVsV; *nɖuma*Kinga; *Kunga; *Kingo*kaga*mEnE; *mAnɛ; *mani*OɲO; *ɔɲɔ; *VɲV*iɭi; *ʈV
Proto-Mangbetu[36]*mʷɔ̀*bɪ́*amɔ̀*kɪ́*kàɖrà*tí(kpɔ̀)*álí*kpɔ̀*kɪ́rɪ́ɛ̀*gʷò*láɲɔ̀*kɛ̀lʊ̀
Mangbutu[37]owékékíubítongiusɛ́kedrúutíkotoikpiokpáuwɛanoaɓé
Bale[37]ɲɔ̌ndǔ̱tú̱datsokpatsúwyɔngbá / nzú
Ndru[37]nikpɔ́ɓi(na)ondǐ̹tsǔ̹kudatsuâzûkpáítsúǐɗáɲúóvôná
Ma'di (Uganda)[38]ɔ̀mvɔ̄lɛ̀ɖátiàrɪ́hʷakʷɛèyíɲā
Birri[39]mɛ́; mʊ́nvö; nvuímɔ̀; ámɔ̀ìnɖrɔ́; ìnɖrátyi(di)ɔ́tɔ́kpɔkpi; kpɪwuɔnyoiri
Kresh[40]mumumbímbiuŋúʃɛ́ʃɛ̀ndjindja-sramakpɔkpɔ́kpikpiùyùɔ́ʃɔ́díri
Dongo[40]mómumbimbiʔɔŋucẹ̀cẹ̀ndjándja-ọọskpọkpŏkpikpiùyùl-ọc(ic)díri
Aja[40]iɲimimbimúmúukundindyi-usagbäbícícíɓaɓakiri
Kunama[41]ùkùˈnàbòbòˈnàŋèeˈlàùˈdàkòkòˈbàsàŋˈgàèˈlàbìˈàˈìŋ(à)ˈkíidà
Berta[42]ařeiileamúŋndu-fuudíhalan'duk’aβak’aaras’ís’íafɪ'riθɪ́ŋahuu (= foot)
Gumuz, Northern[43]kʼwácátsʼéaíítakʼósakʼótʼásamaχáʒákwáɟáajatsʼéa
Proto-Koman[44]*D̪E*cʼɛ*ʃʊnʃ*ʃE*lEtʼ̪a*tʼ̪wa*sʼámá; *bàs*ʃUImakʼ*cwálá*jiɗE*ʃa; *kʼama*D̪uga
Amdang (Kouchane)[45]nidili, kiliŋgɛgʊrnɑkɑlkɑdɔlːsɪˈmitʃoːdʊrtusɔŋsunuzɑmtʃuluk
Maba[46]kàʃì-k/-ñikoi-kboiñsati-kdelmi-kkan-a/-tuàríikàñjí-ksoŋgo-kinjimílí-i/-síi
Mimi of Decorse[47]dyofeɾfirɲainɲyosuengiɲyam
Kanuri[48]shîmsə́mòkə́nzàtímì; shélìtə́làmshíllàkə̀skánjî
Zaghawa[49][50]íkέbέsínámàrgiːtàmsiːááógúúrúbɛ̀gìdiːsε:gìtír
Dendi[51]háŋŋánínèhínydyèdɛ́llɛ̀méèkpííʀìbíʀítúúʀìhàʀíŋwáàmáà
Tadaksahak[52]haŋgát-í-nʒarée-ʃaníilǝsmíyakud-énbiidítugúduaryénŋámân
gollark: Yeß.
gollark: Tapes play at 48k samples per second and DFPWM is 1 bit per sample.
gollark: Also mining.
gollark: I think I tried to deploy one on SC.
gollark: > someone would probably grey goo it thoughFortunately, turtle selfreplicators are VERY SLOW.

See also

References

  1. Campbell, Lyle; Mixco, Mauricio J. (2007). A Glossary of Historical Linguistics. University of Utah Press. ISBN 978-0-87480-892-6.
  2. Matthews, P. H. (2007). Oxford Concise Dictionary of Linguistics (2nd ed.). Oxford. ISBN 978-0-19-920272-0.,
  3. Blench, Roger & Lameen Souag. m.s. Saharan and Songhay form a branch of Nilo-Saharan.
  4. Dimmendaal, Gerrit J. (1992). "Nilo-Saharan Languages". International Encyclopedia of Linguistics. volume 3. Oxford. pp. 100–104. ISBN 0-19-505196-3.
  5. Bender, M. Lionel (2000). "Nilo-Saharan". African Languages, An Introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 43–73. ISBN 0-521-66178-1.
  6. Blench, Roger; Ahland, Colleen (2010). The Classification of Gumuz and Koman Languages. Language Isolates in Africa workshop, Lyons, December 4. Archived from the original on March 16, 2012.
  7. Clark, John Desmond (1984). From Hunters to Farmers: The Causes and Consequences of Food Production in Africa. University of California Press. p. 31. ISBN 0-520-04574-2.
  8. Drake, N. A.; Blench, R. M.; Armitage, S. J.; Bristow, C. S.; White, K. H. (2011). "Ancient watercourses and biogeography of the Sahara explain the peopling of the desert". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 108 (2): 458–62. doi:10.1073/pnas.1012231108. PMC 3021035. PMID 21187416.
  9. Lewis, M. Paul, ed. (2009). "Maasai: A language of Kenya". Ethnologue: Languages of the World (Sixteenth ed.). Dallas, TX: SIL International..
  10. Diedrich Westermann, 1912. The Shilluk people, their language and folklore
  11. Bender, M. Lionel (1991) "Subclassification of Nilo-Saharan". In Bender, M. Lionel, ed. (1991) Proceedings of the Fourth Nilo-Saharan Conference, Bayreuth, Aug. 30–Sep. 2, 1989. Hamburg: Helmut Buske Verlag. NISA 7, 1–36
  12. Roger Blench (2006). The Niger-Saharan Macrophylum (PDF). Cambridge: Mallam Dendo. p. 5.
  13. Bender, Lionel (1996). The Nilo-Saharan languages: a comparative essay. Munich: Lincom Europa.
  14. Bender, Lionel (2000). "Nilo-Saharan". In Heine, Bernd; Nurse, Derek (eds.). African Languages: An Introduction. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-66178-1.
  15. Ehret (2001)
  16. Blench, Roger. 2015. Was there a now-vanished branch of Nilo-Saharan on the Dogon Plateau? Evidence from substrate vocabulary in Bangime and Dogon.
  17. Africa over the last 12,000 years
  18. George Starostin (2016) The Nilo-Saharan hypothesis tested through lexicostatistics: current state of affairs
  19. Languages of Africa: A New Lexicostatistical Classification. Vol. III: Nilo-Saharan Languages
  20. Dimmendaal, Gerrit J. (2016). "On stable and unstable features in Nilo-Saharan". The University of Nairobi Journal of Language and Linguistics.
  21. Gerrit Dimmendaal, Colleen Ahland & Angelika Jakobi (2019) "Linguistic features and typologies in languages commonly referred to as 'Nilo-Saharan'", Cambridge Handbook of African Linguistics
  22. Dimmendaal, Gerrit J. (2011). Historical Linguistics and the Comparative Study of African Languages. John Benjamins. p. 313. ISBN 978-90-272-8722-9.
  23. Blench, Roger M. (2000) "The classification of Nilo-Saharan" (Afrika und Übersee 83). p. 299.
  24. Blench, Roger (2004). "Review of The Civilizations of Africa: A History to 1800". The African Archaeological Review. 21 (4): 239–242. doi:10.1007/s10437-004-0752-7. ISSN 0263-0338. JSTOR 25130809.
  25. Dimmendaal, Gerrit Jan. 1988. "The lexical reconstruction of proto-Nilotic: a first reconnaissance." Afrikanistische (AAP) 16: 5-67.
  26. Bender, M. Lionel. 1998. "The Eastern Jebel Languages of Sudan." Afrika und Übersee 81: 39-64.
  27. Blench, Roger. Temein languages comparative wordlist.
  28. Thelwall, Robin. 1981. The Daju Language Group. Doctoral dissertation. Coleraine: New University of Ulster.
  29. Schadeberg, Thilo. 1994. Comparative Kadu Wordlists. Afrikanistische Arbeitspapiere 40:11-48. University of Cologne.
  30. Rilly, Claude. 2010. Le méroïtique et sa famille linguistique. Leuven: Peeters Publishers.
  31. Yigezu, Moges. 2001. A comparative study of the phonetics and phonology of Surmic languages. Bruxelles: Université libre de Bruxelles. Doctoral dissertation, University of Bruxelles.
  32. Heine, Bernd. 1976. The Kuliak Languages of Eastern Uganda. Nairobi: East African Publishing House.
  33. Jordan, Linda, Hussein Mohammed, and Jillian Netzley. 2015. Sociolinguistic Survey of the Shabo of Ethiopia. SIL Electronic Survey Report 2015-019. SIL International.
  34. Wedekind, Klaus. 2001. Sociolinguistic Survey Report of the Languages of the Gawwada, Tsamay, and Diraasha Areas with Excursions to Birayle (Ongota) and Arbore (Irbore): Part 2. SIL Electronic Survey Reports 2002-066: 6-15.
  35. Boyeldieu, Pascal, Pierre Nougayrol, and Pierre Palayer. 2006. Lexique comparatif historique des langues Sara-Bongo-Baguirmiennes. Online version.
  36. Demolin, Didier. 1992. Le Mangbetu: etude phonétique et phonologique, 2 vols. Brussels: Faculté de Philosophie et Lettres, Université libre de Bruxelles dissertation.
  37. Bokula, Moiso & Agozia-Kario Irumu. 1994. Bibliographie et matériaux lexicaux des langues Moru-Mangbetu (Soudan-Central, Zaïre). Annales Aequatoria 10: 203‒245.
  38. Boone, Douglas; Richard L. Watson (editors). 1996. Moru-Ma'di survey report. Nairobi, Kenya: Summer Institute of Linguistics.
  39. Santandrea, Stefano. 1966. The Birri language: Brief elementary notes. Afrika und Übersee 49: 81‒234.
  40. Santandrea, Stefano. 1976. The Kresh group, Aja and Baka languages (Sudan): A linguistic contribution. Napoli: Istituto Universitario Orientale.
  41. Bender, Lionel. 2001. English-Kunama lexicon. Afrikanistische Arbeitspapiere 65: 201-253.
  42. Bender, M. Lionel. 1989. Berta Lexicon. In Bender, M. Lionel (ed.), Topics in Nilo-Saharan Linguistics, 271-304. Hamburg: Helmut Buske.
  43. Ahland, Colleen and Eliza Kelly. 2014. Daatsʼíin-Gumuz Comparative Word list.
  44. Otero, Manuel Alejandro. 2019. A Historical Reconstruction of the Koman Language Family. Doctoral thesis. Department of Linguistics, University of Oregon.
  45. Wolf, Katharina. 2010. Une enquête sociolinguistique parmi les Amdang (Mimi) du Tchad: Rapport Technique. SIL Electronic Survey Reports 2010-028
  46. Edgar, John T. 1991. Maba-group Lexicon. (Sprache und Oralität in Afrika: Frankfurter Studien zur Afrikanistik, 13.) Berlin: Dietrich Reimer.
  47. Gaudefroy-Demombynes, Maurice. 1907. Document sur les Langues de l'Oubangui-Chari. In Actes du XVIe Congrès International des Orientalistes, Alger, 1905, Part II, 172-330. Paris: Ernest Leroux.
  48. Doris Löhr, H. Ekkehard Wolff (with Ari Awagana). 2009. Kanuri vocabulary. In: Haspelmath, Martin & Tadmor, Uri (eds.) World Loanword Database. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, 1591 entries.
  49. Blažek, Václav. 2007. ]https://www.muni.cz/en/research/publications/763232 On application of Glottochronology for Saharan Languages]. In Viva Africa 2007. Proceedings of the IInd International Conference on African Studies (April 2007). Plzeň: Dryáda, 2007. p. 19-38, 19 pp. ISBN 978-80-87025-17-8.
  50. Tourneux, Henry. 1992. Inventaire phonologiques et formation du pluriel en zaghawa (Tchad). Afrika und Übersee 75, 267-277.
  51. Zima, Petr. 1994. Lexique dendi (songhay): Djougou, Bénin: avec un index français-dendi. (Westafrikanische Studien 4). Köln: Rüdiger Köppe.
  52. Christiansen-Bolli, Regula. 2010. A Grammar of Tadaksahak: a Northern Songhay Language of Mali. Leiden.

Further reading

  • Christopher Ehret (2001). A historical-comparative reconstruction of Nilo-Saharan. Köln: R. Köppe Verlag. ISBN 3-89645-098-0. OCLC 48027016.
  • Dimmendaal, Gerrit J. (2008-09-01). "Language Ecology and Linguistic Diversity on the African Continent". Language and Linguistics Compass. 2 (5): 840–858. doi:10.1111/j.1749-818x.2008.00085.x. ISSN 1749-818X.
  • Joseph Greenberg (1970). Written at International Journal of American Linguistics 29.1. The languages of Africa. Bloomington: Indiana University. ISBN 0-87750-115-7. OCLC 795772769.CS1 maint: location (link)
  • Roger Blench (2006). Archaeology, language, and the African past. Lanham, MD: AltaMira Press. ISBN 0-7591-0465-4. OCLC 62281704.
  • Pertti Mikkola, 1999. "Nilo-Saharan revisited: some observations concerning the best etymologies". Nordic Journal of African Studies, 8(2):108–138.

External relationships

  • Roger Blench, 2011. "Can Sino-Tibetan and Austroasiatic help us understand the evolution of Niger-Congo noun classes?", CALL 41, Leiden
  • Gregersen, Edgar (1972). "Kongo-Saharan". Journal of African Languages. 11 (1): 69–89.
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.