Shooting bias

The term shooting bias, also known as "shooter bias", is a form of implicit racial bias which refers to the tendency among the police to shoot black civilians more often than white civilians, even when they are unarmed.

The concept proposes that the probability of being shot by the police depends on ethnicity in addition to the other known factors like location, the income of the neighborhood and whether or not the person is carrying a weapon[1] as well as the emotions shown by the victim.[2]

Key Questions in Literature

Benchmark

Violent crime is committed by a very small number of individuals. These individuals tend to come from very poor disadvantaged communities. Since minorities are overrepresented in these communities, minorities are also overrepresented in violent crime. Thus, any statistical analysis of police bias must take this into account. Using population, police-citizen interactions, or total arrests as a benchmark, we observe that black citizens appear more likely than white citizens to be fatally shot by police officers in both years. Using violent crime arrests or weapons offense arrests, we observe that black citizens appear less likely to be fatally shot by police officers.[3]

Resisting Arrest

Additionally, data on the percent of individuals who resist arrest is also needed to draw conclusions about police bias. That data was available for Texas and California. Researchers were able to analyze this data to determine that there was shooting bias in California but not in Texas.[4]

Number of deaths

Problems with police reporting practices

Police data is not biased due to police reporting practices.[5] Departments can voluntarily include justifiable homicides in the crime statistics of the FBI's Uniform Crime Reports, which means that a lot of departments don't provide data at all. Some cities haven't reported their data in years.[6] This means that the official data doesn't accurately reflect the number of civilians that are shot by the police.

Newspapers like The Guardian and The Washington Post have started compiling databases of fatal police shootings,[7][8] revealing that in 2015 twice as many civilians had been fatally shot as the FBI's data suggested.[9] An FBI working group has started working on a proposal for making the reports more accurate, but they would still rely on voluntary data and therefore wouldn't fix the main reporting issue.[10]

Alternative database by The Guardian

The database developed by The Guardian is currently the largest database on fatal shootings available.[11] They gather data through police reports, monitoring of regional news, fact-checked witness statements and other crowdsourced police fatality databases.[12]

Number of deaths

In 2016, The Guardian counted 1093 people[7] who were killed by the police in the United States. Out of these, 574 were white and 266 were black. 95 of the white victims were unarmed, while 42 of the black victims were unarmed.

More white than black people are shot. It is important to distinguish to differentiate between the number of deaths of an ethnic group and the likelihood of being shot by police. The likelihood of being shot as a black rather than a white person is higher, whether the victim is armed or not.[1]

Likelihood of being shot: Black vs. White

A study carried out at the University of California found "evidence of a significant bias in the killing of unarmed black Americans compared to unarmed white Americans".[1] In this study, the probability of being shot by the police as a black, unarmed person versus as a white, unarmed person was 3.49 times higher. Unarmed Hispanics' likelihood to be shot was 1.67 times higher than for unarmed Whites. However, these stats do not reflect that people get shot when coming into contact with the police which is usually whilst carrying out a violent criminal act or being arrested for a criminal act. Black men represent around 6% of the population but account for around 52.6% of murder arrests and 54.5% of robbery arrests.[13] Even so they only represent 28% of people killed by police whilst non blacks suffer 72% of the people shot and killed for less than half the crimes.[14]

A more recent study was conducted by Michigan State University and the University of Maryland, compiling a list of more than 900 fatal U.S. police shootings in 2015 using crowdsourced databases from The Washington Post and The Guardian. Then, they asked police departments for information about the race of the officers responsible for the shootings. They found black police were more likely to kill black civilians than white civilians. However, the same held true for white and Hispanic officers: Each group of police was more likely to shoot civilians of their own race. Researchers claimed this is true because police tend to be drawn from the communities they work in and are thus more likely to have deadly encounters with civilians of the same race. They conclude that "increasing diversity among officers by itself is unlikely to reduce racial disparity in police shootings".

Factors that influence decision to shoot

List of empirically researched factors

Empirical research suggests the following factors to influence the decision to shoot:

  • Implicit racial bias, prejudice, and stereotypes[15]
  • Status armed/unarmed[1]
  • Gender (men are more likely to be shot)[16]
  • County[1]
  • Income and status of the neighbourhood[17][18]
  • Previous experiences of the police officers with the ethnic group[19]
  • Previous experiences of the victim with the police[19]
  • Emotional facial expression of the civilian[2]
  • Fatigue of the police officer[20]

Implicit racial bias

An implicit racial bias refers to unintentional judgments a person makes of a group (e.g. good/bad) of a certain ethnicity. So a person who shows implicit racial bias might not be aware of it. Police officers have been found to show a racial bias against black people in the decision to shoot.[21]

In one of the studies, researchers investigated how stereotypes affected police officers' decisions to shoot. They used a video game and exposed their participants to pictures of either Whites or Non-Whites who were armed or unarmed. During this video game, the participants were asked to choose between "shoot" or "don't shoot" as quickly as possible. The results were that the participants shot armed black people faster than armed white people and chose "don't shoot" faster for unarmed white than unarmed black persons.

The authors explained those findings by the activation of stereotype thinking which lead white people to associate black people with danger. Because of this stereotype, the participants expected blacks to carry a gun and therefore were quicker to make the "shooting" decision.[15] Other studies have found similar results.[22][23] The time pressure to make a decision in decisions to shoot might magnify the effects of racial bias.[24] However, more recent research indicates that despite scoring moderately high on an Implicit Association Test, police officers are less likely to shoot black people in a video simulation[25]

Location and context

The American police officers live and work in a society full of prejudices against minorities. In addition, police officers usually deal with high crime rates in minority neighborhoods. These experiences reinforce their existing prejudices by ignoring that most people with a non-white ethnic background don't become criminals. This leads to discrimination against minorities. Other factors that lead to discrimination by the police are institutionalized language barriers between police and some ethnic groups, experiences with disrespectful or hostile residents in certain minority neighborhoods and low punishment for police officers who misbehave towards minorities.

Those prejudices by the police are of course being noticed by the groups of minorities. So in return, the groups believe that the officers have prejudices against them. This might lead to the findings that Non-Whites are more likely to behave disrespectfully or to insult officers.

To sum up, there may exist a mutual influence between the prejudices that police officers have against minorities and the prejudices that minorities have against officers.[19]

Examples that showcase prejudices among police officers[19]
  • Disruptive actions by the police like stopping and questioning citizens on the street are more common in minority neighborhoods
  • More cases of Non-Whites are being dropped than of Whites. This may be because the reasons for arresting them were too weak or unjustified in the first place.

Ways to reduce shooting bias

Possibly the biggest change could be made by holding police officers accountable for their actions.[26] Internal investigations have been criticized for bias in favor of officers, leading to a lack of punishment. The work culture within some police departments has been criticized as racist, in light of many recent scandals.[27][28][29]

Knowing the factors that influence police officers decisions to shoot there are a few possible solutions for reducing shooting bias. The main factor is implicit racial bias, which in turn is exuberated by certain factors that could be addressed.

Diversity in police departments might not reduce shooting bias,[30] but reducing fatigue might lower the impact of racial bias on the decision to shoot.[20] Changing the training of police officers so as to not showcase black armed targets more often than white ones could help reduce racial bias.[31][32]

Training police officers in making decisions under stress as well as assigning officers to certain locations so they can become familiar with its residents could reduce the numbers of civilians killed, says Richmond's police chief, Chris Magnus.[33]

Racial bias can be contagious within a social group or neighborhood.[34][35] Training police officers to be aware of this might help in reducing this effect.

gollark: ++remind 3.5h bee bee apiforl.
gollark: https://www.reddit.com/r/SubSimulatorGPT2/comments/ol75o4/eli5_why_we_use_numbers_in_a_computer_programmer/
gollark: I shan't.
gollark: Interesting fact: Feature wise, AV1 is specifically designed for real-time applications (especially WebRTC) and higher resolutions (wider color gamuts, higher frame rates, UHD) than typical usage scenarios of the current generation (H.264) of video formats, where it is expected to achieve its biggest efficiency gains. It is therefore planned to support the color space from ITU-R Recommendation BT.2020 and up to 12 bits of precision per color component. AV1 is primarily intended for lossy encoding, although lossless compression is supported as well.
gollark: Does anyone?

References

  1. Ross, Cody T. (2015-11-05). "A Multi-Level Bayesian Analysis of Racial Bias in Police Shootings at the County-Level in the United States, 2011–2014". PLoS ONE. 10 (11): e0141854. Bibcode:2015PLoSO..1041854R. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0141854. PMC 4634878. PMID 26540108.
  2. Kubota, Jennifer T.; Ito, Tiffany A. (2014). "The role of expression and race in weapons identification". Emotion. 14 (6): 1115–1124. doi:10.1037/a0038214. PMC 4235656. PMID 25401289.
  3. Tregle, Brandon; Nix, Justin; Alpert, Geoffrey P. (2018-12-07). "Disparity does not mean bias: making sense of observed racial disparities in fatal officer-involved shootings with multiple benchmarks". Journal of Crime and Justice. 42 (1): 18–31. doi:10.1080/0735648x.2018.1547269. ISSN 0735-648X.
  4. Shjarback, John A.; Nix, Justin (January 2020). "Considering violence against police by citizen race/ethnicity to contextualize representation in officer-involved shootings". Journal of Criminal Justice. 66: 101653. doi:10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2019.101653. ISSN 0047-2352.
  5. Klinger, David A. (2011-12-13). "On the Problems and Promise of Research on Lethal Police Violence". Homicide Studies. 16 (1): 78–96. doi:10.1177/1088767911430861.
  6. Byers, Christine; Moskop, Walker (2014-11-09). "Nobody counts police killings in the U.S." stltoday.com. Retrieved 2017-07-30.
  7. Swaine, Jon; Laughland, Oliver; Lartey, Jamiles; Davis, Kenan; Harris, Rich; Popovich, Nadja; Powell, Kenton; team, Guardian US interactive. "The Counted: people killed by police in the United States – interactive". the Guardian. Retrieved 2017-07-30.
  8. "Police shootings 2017 database". Washington Post. Retrieved 2017-07-30.
  9. Kindy, Kimberly; et al. (2015-05-30). "Fatal police shootings in 2015 approaching 400 nationwide". Washington Post. Retrieved 2019-12-08.
  10. Kindy, Kimberly (2015-12-08). "FBI to sharply expand system for tracking fatal police shootings". Washington Post. Retrieved 2019-12-08.
  11. Hehman, Eric; Flake, Jessica K.; Calanchini, Jimmy (2017-07-27). "Disproportionate Use of Lethal Force in Policing Is Associated With Regional Racial Biases of Residents". Social Psychological and Personality Science. 9 (4): 393–401. doi:10.1177/1948550617711229.
  12. "About The Counted: why and how the Guardian is counting US police killings". the Guardian. Retrieved 2017-07-30.
  13. https://ucr.fbi.gov/crime-in-the-u.s/2016/crime-in-the-u.s.-2016/topic-pages/tables/table-21
  14. https://mappingpoliceviolence.org/
  15. Correll, Joshua; Park, Bernadette; Judd, Charles M.; Wittenbrink, Bernd (2007). "The influence of stereotypes on decisions to shoot". European Journal of Social Psychology. 37 (6): 1102–1117. doi:10.1002/ejsp.450.
  16. Sagara, Ryan Gabrielson, Ryann Grochowski Jones, Eric (2014-10-10). "Deadly Force, in Black and White". ProPublica. Retrieved 2017-07-30.
  17. Smith, Douglas A. (1986). "The Neighborhood Context of Police Behavior". Crime and Justice. 8: 313–341. doi:10.1086/449126.
  18. Terrill, William; Reisig, Michael D. (2016-08-18). "Neighborhood Context and Police Use of Force". Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. 40 (3): 291–321. doi:10.1177/0022427803253800.
  19. Weitzer, Ronald (1996). "Racial discrimination in the criminal justice system: Findings and problems in the literature". Journal of Criminal Justice. 24 (4): 309–322. doi:10.1016/0047-2352(96)00015-3.
  20. Ma, Debbie S.; Correll, Joshua; Wittenbrink, Bernd; Bar-Anan, Yoav; Sriram, N.; Nosek, Brian A. (2013-11-01). "When Fatigue Turns Deadly: The Association Between Fatigue and Racial Bias in the Decision to Shoot". Basic and Applied Social Psychology. 35 (6): 515–524. doi:10.1080/01973533.2013.840630.
  21. Correll, Joshua; Park, Bernadette; Judd, Charles M.; Wittenbrink, Bernd; Sadler, Melody S.; Keesee, Tracie (2007). "Across the thin blue line: Police officers and racial bias in the decision to shoot". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 92 (6): 1006–1023. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1006. PMID 17547485.
  22. Correll, Joshua; Wittenbrink, Bernd; Crawford, Matthew T.; Sadler, Melody S. (2015). "Stereotypic vision: How stereotypes disambiguate visual stimuli". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 108 (2): 219–233. doi:10.1037/pspa0000015. PMID 25603373.
  23. Payne, B. Keith (2001). "Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81 (2): 181–192. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.181.
  24. Payne, B. Keith (2006-12-01). "Weapon Bias: Split-Second Decisions and Unintended Stereotyping". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 15 (6): 287–291. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00454.x.
  25. James, Lois; James, Stephen M.; Vila, Bryan J. (2016-01-14). "The Reverse Racism Effect". Criminology & Public Policy. 15 (2): 457–479. doi:10.1111/1745-9133.12187. ISSN 1538-6473.
  26. Hudson, Redditt (2014-12-06). "Being a cop showed me just how racist and violent the police are. There's only one fix". Washington Post. Retrieved 2017-07-30.
  27. "Michigan police suspend cop behind 'racist' footage of black man". NY Daily News. Retrieved 2017-07-30.
  28. "Ohio cops suspended for exchanging racist text messages". NY Daily News. Retrieved 2017-07-30.
  29. "Two Black Cops Write Statements About Racism in LAPD". BET.com. Retrieved 2017-07-30.
  30. Smith, Brad W. (2003-05-01). "The Impact of Police Officer Diversity on Police-Caused Homicides". Policy Studies Journal. 31 (2): 147–162. doi:10.1111/1541-0072.t01-1-00009.
  31. Sim, Jessica J.; Correll, Joshua; Sadler, Melody S. (2013-03-01). "Understanding Police and Expert Performance: When Training Attenuates (vs. Exacerbates) Stereotypic Bias in the Decision to Shoot". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 39 (3): 291–304. doi:10.1177/0146167212473157. PMID 23401478.
  32. Plant, E. Ashby; Peruche, B. Michelle; Butz, David A. (2005). "Eliminating automatic racial bias: Making race non-diagnostic for responses to criminal suspects". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 41 (2): 141–156. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2004.07.004.
  33. Lind, Dara (2014-09-18). "How to not shoot civilians: 9 community policing tips from a chief who got it right". Vox. Retrieved 2017-07-30.
  34. Weisbuch, Max; Pauker, Kristin (2011-12-01). "The Nonverbal Transmission of Intergroup Bias: A Model of Bias Contagion with Implications for Social Policy". Social Issues and Policy Review. 5 (1): 257–291. doi:10.1111/j.1751-2409.2011.01032.x. PMC 3756600. PMID 23997812.
  35. Weisbuch, Max; Pauker, Kristin; Ambady, Nalini (2009-12-18). "The Subtle Transmission of Race Bias via Televised Nonverbal Behavior". Science. 326 (5960): 1711–1714. Bibcode:2009Sci...326.1711W. doi:10.1126/science.1178358. PMC 3764987. PMID 20019288.
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.