Heterochrony

In evolutionary developmental biology, heterochrony is any genetically controlled difference in the timing or duration of a developmental process in an organism compared to its ancestors or other organisms. This leads to changes in the size, shape, characteristics and even presence of certain organs and features. It is contrasted with heterotopy, a change in spatial positioning of some process in the embryo, which can also create morphological innovation. Heterochrony can be divided into intraspecific heterochrony, variation within a species, and interspecific heterochrony, phylogenetic variation, i.e. variation of a descendant species with respect to an ancestral species.[2]

Giraffes acquired their long necks through heterochrony, extending the development period of the seven neck vertebrae growth in the embryo to add length to the bones, not by adding more bones.[1]

These changes all affect the start, end, rate or time span of a particular developmental process.[3] The concept of heterochrony was introduced by Ernst Haeckel in 1875[4][5] and given its modern sense by Gavin de Beer in 1930.

History

Ernst Haeckel supposed that embryonic development recapitulated an animal's phylogeny, and introduced heterochrony as an exception for individual organs. Modern biology agrees instead with Karl Ernst von Baer's view that development itself varies, such as by changing the timing of different processes, to cause a branching phylogeny.[6]

The concept of heterochrony was introduced by the German zoologist Ernst Haeckel in 1875, where he used it to define deviations from recapitulation theory, which held that "ontogeny recapitulates phylogeny".[4] As Stephen Jay Gould pointed out, Haeckel's term is now used in a sense contrary to his coinage. He assumed that embryonic development (ontogeny) of "higher" animals recapitulated their ancestral development (phylogeny). This, in his view, necessarily compressed the earlier developmental stages, representing the ancestors, into a shorter time, meaning accelerated development. The ideal for Haeckel would be when the development of every part of an organism was thus accelerated, but he recognised that some organs could develop with displacements in position (heterotopy, another concept he originated) or time (heterochrony), as exceptions to his rule. He thus intended the term to mean a change in the timing of the embryonic development of one organ with respect to the rest of the same animal, whereas it is now used, following the work of the British evolutionary embryologist Gavin de Beer in 1930, to mean a change with respect to the development of the same organ in the animal's ancestors.[7][8]

In 1928, the English embryologist Walter Garstang showed that tunicate larvae shared structures such as the notochord with adult vertebrates, and suggested that the vertebrates arose by paedomorphosis (neoteny) from such a larva.[9]

De Beer anticipated evolutionary developmental biology in his 1930 book Embryos and Ancestors,[10] showing that evolution could occur by heterochrony, such as in paedomorphosis, the retention of juvenile features in the adult.[11][5] De Beer argued that this enabled rapid evolutionary change, too brief to be recorded in the fossil record, and in effect explaining why apparent gaps were likely.[12]

Mechanisms

Diagram of the six types of shift in heterochrony, a change in the timing or rate of any process in embryonic development. Predisplacement, hypermorphosis, and acceleration extend development (peramorphosis, in red); postdisplacement, hypomorphosis, and deceleration all truncate it (paedomorphosis, in blue). These may be combined, e.g. to shift some aspect of development earlier.[2]

Heterochrony can be divided into intraspecific and interspecific types.

Intraspecific heterochrony means changes in the rate or timing of development within a species. For example, some individuals of the salamander species Ambystoma talpoideum delay the metamorphosis of the skull.[13] Reilly et al argue we can define these variant individuals as paedotypic (with truncated development relative to the ancestral condition), peratypic (with extended development relative to the ancestral condition), or isotypic (reaching the same ancestral shape, but via a different mechanism).[2]

Interspecific heterochrony means differences in the rate or timing of a descendant species relative to its ancestor. This can result in either paedomophosis (truncating the ancestral ontogeny), peramorphosis (extending past the ancestral ontogeny), or isomorphosis (reaching the same ancestral state via a different mechanism).[2]

There are three major mechanisms of heterochrony,[14][15][16][17] each of which can change in either of two directions, giving six types of perturbations, which can be combined in various ways.[18] These ultimately result in extended, shifted, or truncated development of a particular process, such as the action of a single toolkit gene,[19] relative to the ancestral condition or to other conspecifics, depending on whether inter- or intraspecific heterochrony is the focus. Identifying which of the six perturbations is occurring is critical in identifying the actual underlying mechanism driving peramorphosis or paedomorphosis.[2]

Despite greatly differing neck lengths, giraffes (right) have no more cervical vertebrae, just 7, than their fellow giraffids, okapi (left). With the number constrained, the development of the vertebrae is extended, allowing them to grow longer.
  • Onset: A developmental process can either begin earlier, pre-displacement, extending its development, or later, post-displacement, truncating it.
  • Offset: A process can either end later, hypermorphosis, extending its development, or earlier, hypomorphosis or progenesis, truncating it.
  • Rate: The rate of a process can accelerate, extending its development, or decelerate (as in neoteny), truncating it.

A dramatic illustration of how acceleration can change a body plan is seen in snakes. Where a typical vertebrate like a mouse has only around 60 vertebrae, snakes have between around 150 to 400, giving them extremely long spinal columns and enabling their sinuous locomotion. Snake embryos achieve this by accelerating their system for creating somites (body segments), which relies on an oscillator. The oscillator clock runs some four times faster in snake than in mouse embryos, initially creating very thin somites. These expand to adopt a typical vertebrate shape, elongating the body.[20] Giraffes gain their long necks by a different heterochrony, extending the development of their cervical vertebrae; they retain the usual mammalian number of these vertebrae, seven.[1] This number appears to be constrained by the use of neck somites to form the mammalian diaphragm muscle; the result is that the embryonic neck is divided into three modules, the middle one (C3 to C5) serving the diaphragm. The assumption is that disrupting this would kill the embryo rather than giving it more vertebrae.[21]

Detection

Heterochrony can be identified by comparing phylogenetically close species, for example a group of different bird species whose legs differ in their average length. These comparisons are complex because there are no universal ontogenetic timemarkers. The method of event pairing attempts to overcome this by comparing the relative timing of two events at a time.[22] This method detects event heterochronies, as opposed to allometric changes. It is cumbersome to use because the number of event pair characters increases with the square of the number of events compared. Event pairing can however be automated, for instance with the PARSIMOV script.[23] A recent method, continuous analysis, rests on a simple standardization of ontogenetic time or sequences, on squared change parsimony and phylogenetic independent contrasts.[24]

Effects

Paedomorphosis

Axolotls retain gills and fins as adults; these are juvenile features in most amphibians.

Paedomorphosis can be the result of neoteny, the retention of juvenile traits into the adult form as a result of retardation of somatic development, or of progenesis, the acceleration of developmental processes such that the juvenile form becomes a sexually mature adult.[25]

Neoteny retards the development of the organism into an adult, and has been described as "eternal childhood".[26] In this form of heterochrony, the developmental stage of childhood is itself extended, and certain developmental processes that normally take place only during childhood (such as accelerated brain growth in humans[27][28][29]), is also extended throughout this period. Neoteny has been implicated as a developmental cause for a number of behavior changes, as a result of increased brain plasticity and extended childhood.[30]

Progenesis (or paedogenesis) can be observed in the axolotl (Ambystoma mexicanum). Axolotls reach full sexual maturity while retaining their fins and gills (in other words, still in the juvenile form of their ancestors). They will remain in aquatic environments in this truncated developmental form, rather than moving onto land as other sexually mature salamander species. This is thought to be a form of hypomorphosis (earlier ending of development)[31] that is both hormonally[32][33] and genetically driven.[32] The entire metamorphosis that would allow the salamander to transition into the adult form is essentially blocked by both of these drivers.[34]

Paedomorphosis may play a critical role in avian cranial evolution.[35] The skulls and beaks of living, adult birds retain the anatomy of the juvenile theropod dinosaurs from which they evolved.[36] Extant birds have large eyes and brains relative to the rest of the skull; a condition seen in adult birds that represents (broadly speaking) the juvenile stage of a dinosaur.[37] A juvenile avian ancestor (as typified by Coelophysis ) would have a short face, large eyes, a thin palate, narrow jugal bone, tall and thin postorbitals, restricted adductors, and a short and bulbous braincase. As an organism such as this aged, they would change greatly in their cranial morphology to develop a robust skull with larger, overlapping bones. Birds, however, retain this juvenille morphology [38] Evidence from molecular experiments suggests both fibroblast growth factor 8 (FGF8) and members of the WNT signalling pathway have facilitated paedomorphosis in birds.[39] These signalling pathways are known to play roles in facial patterning in other vertebrate species.[40] This retention of the juvenile ancestral state has driven other changes in the anatomy that result in a light, highly kinetic (moveable) skull composed of many small, non-overlapping bones.[38][41] This is believed to have facilitated the evolution of cranial kinesis in birds[38] which has played a critical role in their ecological success.[41]

Peramorphosis

Irish elk skeleton with antlers spanning 2.7 metres (8.9 ft) and a mass of 40 kg (88 lb)

Peramorphosis is delayed maturation with extended periods of growth. An example is the extinct Irish elk. From the fossil record, its antlers spanned up to 12 feet wide, which is about a third larger than the antlers of its close relative the moose. The Irish elk had larger antlers due to extended development during their period of growth.[42][43]

Another example of peramorphosis is seen in insular (island) rodents. Their characteristics include gigantism, wider cheek and teeth, reduced litter size, and longer lifespan. Their relatives that inhabit continental environments are much smaller. Insular rodents have evolved these features to accommodate the abundance of food and resources they have on their islands. These factors are part of a complex phenomenon termed Island syndrome.[44] With less predation and competition for resources, selection favored overdevelopment of these species. Reduced litter sizes enable overdevelopment of their bodies into larger ones.

A 1901 comparison of a frog tadpole (a vertebrate) and a tunicate larva; in 1928 Walter Garstang proposed that vertebrates derived from such a larva by neoteny.

The mole salamander, a close relative to the axolotl, displays both paedomorphosis and paramorphosis. The larva can develop in either direction. Population density, food, and the amount of water may have an effect on the expression of heterochrony. A study conducted on the mole salamander in 1987 found it evident that a higher percentage of individuals became paedomorphic when there was a low larval population density in a constant water level as opposed to a high larval population density in drying water.[45] This had an implication that led to hypotheses that selective pressures imposed by the environment, such as predation and loss of resources, were instrumental to the cause of these trends.[46] These ideas were reinforced by other studies, such as peramorphosis in the Puerto Rican Tree frog. Another reason could be generation time, or the lifespan of the species in question. When a species has a relatively short lifespan, natural selection favors evolution of paedomorphosis (e.g. Axolotl: 7–10 years). Conversely, in long lifespans natural selection favors evolution of peramorphosis (e.g. Irish Elk: 20–22 years).[44]

Across the animal kingdom

Heterochrony is responsible for a wide variety of effects[3] such as the lengthening of the fingers by adding extra phalanges in dolphins to form their flippers,[47] sexual dimorphism,[9] the neotenous origin of the vertebrates from a tunicate larva,[9] and the polymorphism seen between insect castes.[48]

Neoteny in human development

In humans

Several heterochronies have been described in humans, relative to the chimpanzee. In chimpanzee fetuses, brain and head growth starts at about the same developmental stage and grow at a rate similar to that of humans, but growth stops soon after birth, whereas humans continue brain and head growth several years after birth. This particular type of heterochrony, hypermorphosis, involves a delay in the offset of a developmental process, or what is the same, the presence of an early developmental process in later stages of development. Humans have some 30 different neotenies in comparison to the chimpanzee, retaining larger heads, smaller jaws and noses, and shorter limbs, features found in juvenile chimpanzees.[49][50]

The term "heterokairy" was proposed in 2003 by John Spicer and Warren Burggren to distinguish plasticity in timing of the onset of developmental events at the level of an individual or population.[51]

gollark: You should be able to just change the nim version it asks for.
gollark: It has to store historical uptime data for things.
gollark: `sqlite3 monitoring.sqlite3`
gollark: Just change variables to what you want. Although to configure monitoring targets you have to edit the database.
gollark: I MAY integrate an actual command line parser soon™.

See also

References

  1. Hillis, David M. (May 2011). Principles of Life. Palgrave Macmillan. pp. 280–. ISBN 978-1-4641-6298-5.
  2. Reilly, Stephen M.; Wiley, E.O.; Meinhardt, Daniel J. (1997-01-01). "An integrative approach to heterochrony: the distinction between interspecific and intraspecific phenomena". Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 60 (1): 119–143. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.1997.tb01487.x.
  3. Held, Lewis I. (2014). How the Snake Lost its Legs. Curious Tales from the Frontier of Evo-Devo. Cambridge University Press. p. 152. ISBN 978-1-107-62139-8.
  4. Horder, Tim (April 2006). "Heterochrony". Encyclopedia of Life Sciences. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
  5. Hall, B. K. (2003). "Evo-Devo: evolutionary developmental mechanisms". International Journal of Developmental Biology. 47 (7–8): 491–495. PMID 14756324.
  6. Hall, B. K. (2003). "Evo-Devo: evolutionary developmental mechanisms". International Journal of Developmental Biology. 47 (7–8): 491–495. PMID 14756324.
  7. Gould, Stephen J. (1988). "The Uses of Heterochrony". In McKinney, M.L. (ed.). Heterochrony in Evolution. Heterochrony in Evolution. Topics in Geobiology, vol 7. Topics in Geobiology. 7. Springer. pp. 1–13. doi:10.1007/978-1-4899-0795-0_1. ISBN 978-1-4899-0797-4.
  8. Zelditch, Miriam L.; Fink, William L. (2015). "Heterochrony and heterotopy: stability and innovation in the evolution of form". Paleobiology. 22 (2): 241–254. doi:10.1017/S0094837300016195.
  9. McNamara, Kenneth J. (2012). "Heterochrony: the Evolution of Development". Evolution: Education and Outreach. 5 (2): 203–218. doi:10.1007/s12052-012-0420-3.
  10. Held, Lewis I. (2014). How the Snake Lost its Legs. Curious Tales from the Frontier of Evo-Devo. Cambridge University Press. p. 67. ISBN 978-1-107-62139-8.
  11. Gould, Stephen Jay (1977). Ontogeny and Phylogeny. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. pp. 221–222. ISBN 978-0-674-63940-9.
  12. Ingo Brigandt (2006). "Homology and heterochrony: the evolutionary embryologist Gavin Rylands de Beer (1899-1972)" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Zoology. 306B (4): 317–328. doi:10.1002/jez.b.21100. PMID 16506229.
  13. Reilly, Stephen M. (February 1987). "Ontogeny of the hyobranchial apparatus in the salamanders Ambystoma talpoideum (Ambystomatidae) and Notophthalmus viridescens (Salamandridae): The ecological morphology of two neotenic strategies". Journal of Morphology. 191 (2): 205–214. doi:10.1002/jmor.1051910210. PMID 29921111.
  14. Gould, Stephen Jay (1977). Ontogeny and phylogeny. Cambridge, Mass.: Belknap Press. ISBN 978-0674639416. OCLC 2508336.
  15. Keller,Evelyn Fox; Lloyd, Elisabeth A., eds. (1992). Keywords in Evolutionary Biology. Harvard University Press. pp. 158–167. ISBN 978-0674503120.
  16. Alberch, Pedro (1982). Evolution and Development: report of the Dahlem Workshop on Evolution and Development, Berlin 1981, May 10-15. Springer. pp. 313–332. ISBN 978-0387113319.
  17. Fink, William L. (July 1982). "The conceptual relationship between ontogeny and phylogeny". Paleobiology. 8 (3): 254–264. doi:10.1017/s0094837300006977.
  18. Klingenberg, Christian Peter; Spence, John R. (December 1993). "Heterochrony and Allometry: Lessons from the Water Strider Genus Limnoporus". Evolution. 47 (6): 1834–1853. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.1993.tb01273.x. PMID 28567993.
  19. Carroll, Sean B. (2008). "Evo-Devo and an Expanding Evolutionary Synthesis: A Genetic Theory of Morphological Evolution". Cell. 134 (1): 25–36. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2008.06.030. PMID 18614008.
  20. Held, Lewis I. (2014). How the Snake Lost its Legs. Curious Tales from the Frontier of Evo-Devo. Cambridge University Press. pp. 81–84. ISBN 978-1-107-62139-8.
  21. Held, Lewis I. (2014). How the Snake Lost its Legs. Curious Tales from the Frontier of Evo-Devo. Cambridge University Press. pp. 126–127. ISBN 978-1-107-62139-8.
  22. Velhagen, W. A. (1997). "Analyzing developmental sequences using sequences units". Systematic Biology. 46 (1): 204–210. doi:10.1093/sysbio/46.1.204. JSTOR 2413644. PMID 11975352.
  23. Jeffery, J. E.; Bininda-Emonds, O.R.P.; Coates, M.I.; Richardson, M.K. (2005). "A new technique for identifying sequence heterochrony". Systematic Biology. 54 (2): 230–240. doi:10.1080/10635150590923227. PMID 16012094.
  24. Germain, D.; Laurin, M. (2009). "Evolution of ossification sequences in salamanders and urodele origins assessed through event-pairing and new methods". Evolution & Development. 11 (2): 170–190. doi:10.1111/j.1525-142X.2009.00318.x. PMID 19245549.
  25. Smith, Charles Kay (1990). "A model for understanding the evolution of mammalian behavior". Current Mammalogy. 2: 335–374.
  26. Clive., Bromhall (2004-01-01). The eternal child : how evolution has made children of us all. Ebury. ISBN 978-0-091894429. OCLC 59288040.
  27. Somel, Mehmet; Franz, Henriette; Yan, Zheng; Lorenc, Anna; Guo, Song; Giger, Thomas; Kelso, Janet; Nickel, Birgit; Dannemann, Michael (2009-04-07). "Transcriptional neoteny in the human brain". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 106 (14): 5743–5748. doi:10.1073/pnas.0900544106. PMC 2659716. PMID 19307592.
  28. Petanjek, Zdravko; Judaš, Miloš; Šimić, Goran; Rašin, Mladen Roko; Uylings, Harry B. M.; Rakic, Pasko; Kostović, Ivica (2011-08-09). "Extraordinary neoteny of synaptic spines in the human prefrontal cortex". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 108 (32): 13281–13286. doi:10.1073/pnas.1105108108. PMC 3156171. PMID 21788513.
  29. Goyal, Manu S.; Hawrylycz, Michael; Miller, Jeremy A.; Snyder, Abraham Z.; Raichle, Marcus E. (January 2014). "Aerobic Glycolysis in the Human Brain is Associated with Development and Neotenous Gene Expression". Cell Metabolism. 19 (1): 49–57. doi:10.1016/j.cmet.2013.11.020. PMC 4389678. PMID 24411938.
  30. Coppinger, R.; Glendinning, J.; Torop, E.; Matthay, C.; Sutherland, M.; Smith, C. (January 1987). "Degree of Behavioral Neoteny Differentiates Canid Polymorphs". Ethology. 75 (2): 89–108. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1987.tb00645.x.
  31. McKinney, Michael L. (2013-11-21). Heterochrony in evolution : a multidisciplinary approach. McKinney, Michael L. New York. ISBN 978-1489907950. OCLC 883381554.
  32. Tompkins, Robert (May 1978). "Genie Control of Axolotl Metamorphosis". American Zoologist. 18 (2): 313–319. doi:10.1093/icb/18.2.313.
  33. Etkin, William (1968). Metamorphosis a problem in developmental biology. Gilbert, Lawrence Irwin. Appleton-Century-Crofts. ISBN 978-1461332466. OCLC 682061358.
  34. Wainwright, Peter C. (1994). Ecological morphology : integrative organismal biology. University of Chicago Press. p. 324. ISBN 978-0-226869957. OCLC 29357502.
  35. Bhullar, Bhart-Anjan S.; Hanson, Michael; Fabbri, Matteo; Pritchard, Adam; Bever, Gabe S.; Hoffman, Eva (September 2016). "How to Make a Bird Skull: Major Transitions in the Evolution of the Avian Cranium, Paedomorphosis, and the Beak as a Surrogate Hand". Integrative and Comparative Biology. 56 (3): 389–403. doi:10.1093/icb/icw069. PMID 27371392.
  36. Bhullar, Bhart-Anjan S.; Marugán-Lobón, Jesús; Racimo, Fernando; Bever, Gabe S.; Rowe, Timothy B.; Norell, Mark A.; Abzhanov, Arhat (2012-05-27). "Birds have paedomorphic dinosaur skulls". Nature. 487 (7406): 223–226. doi:10.1038/nature11146. PMID 22722850.
  37. Zusi, R. L. (1993). The skull, vol 2: patterns of structural and systematic diversity. University of Chicago Press. pp. 391–437.
  38. Bhullar, Bhart-Anjan S.; Hanson, Michael; Fabbri, Matteo; Pritchard, Adam; Bever, Gabe S.; Hoffman, Eva (2016-09-01). "How to Make a Bird Skull: Major Transitions in the Evolution of the Avian Cranium, Paedomorphosis, and the Beak as a Surrogate Hand". Integrative and Comparative Biology. 56 (3): 389–403. doi:10.1093/icb/icw069. PMID 27371392.
  39. Bhullar, Bhart-Anjan S.; Morris, Zachary S.; Sefton, Elizabeth M.; Tok, Atalay; Tokita, Masayoshi; Namkoong, Bumjin; Camacho, Jasmin; Burnham, David A.; Abzhanov, Arhat (2015-07-01). "A molecular mechanism for the origin of a key evolutionary innovation, the bird beak and palate, revealed by an integrative approach to major transitions in vertebrate history". Evolution. 69 (7): 1665–1677. doi:10.1111/evo.12684. PMID 25964090.
  40. Hu, Diane; Marcucio, Ralph S.; Helms, Jill A. (May 2003). "A zone of frontonasal ectoderm regulates patterning and growth in the face". Development. 130 (9): 1749–1758. doi:10.1242/dev.00397. PMID 12642481.
  41. Bout, Ron G.; Zweers, Gart A (2001). "The role of cranial kinesis in birds". Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology. 131 (1): 197–205. doi:10.1016/s1095-6433(01)00470-6. PMID 11733177.
  42. Futuyma, Douglas (2013). Evolution. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates. pp. 64–66. ISBN 978-1605351155.
  43. Ron A. Moen; John Pastor; Yosef Cohen (1999). "Antler growth and extinction of Irish elk". Evolutionary Ecology Research. 1 (2): 235–249.
  44. Pasquale Raia; Fabio M Guarino; Mimmo Turano; Gianluca Polese; Daniela Rippa; Francesco Carotenuto; Daria M Monti; Manuela Cardi; Domenico Fulgione; et al. (2010). "The blue lizard spandrel and the island syndrome". BMC Evolutionary Biology. 10 (1): 289. doi:10.1186/1471-2148-10-289. PMC 2949876. PMID 20854657.
  45. Semlitsch, Raymond D. (1987). "Paedomorphosis in Ambystoma talpoideum: effects of density, food, and pond drying". Ecology. 68 (4): 992–1002. doi:10.2307/1938370. JSTOR 1938370.
  46. M. Denoel; P. Joly (2000). "Neoteny and progenesis as two heterochronic processes involved in paedomorphosis in Triturus alpestris (Amphibia: Caudata)". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. B. 267 (1451): 1481–1485. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1168. PMC 1690691. PMID 10983835.
  47. Richardson, Michael K.; Oelschlager, Helmut H. A. (2002). "Time, pattern, and heterochrony: a study of hyperphalangy in the dolphin embryo flipper". Evolution and Development. 4 (6): 435–444. doi:10.1046/j.1525-142X.2002.02032.x.
  48. Emlen, Douglas J.; Nijhout, H. Frederik (2000). "The Development and Evolution of Exaggerated Morphologies in Insects". Annual Review of Entomology. 45 (1): 661–708. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.323.5456. doi:10.1146/annurev.ento.45.1.661. PMID 10761593.
  49. Mitteroecker, P.; Gunz, P.; Bernhard, M.; Schaefer, K.; Bookstein, F. L. (June 2004). "Comparison of cranial ontogenetic trajectories among great apes and humans". J. Hum. Evol. 46 (6): 679–97. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2004.03.006. PMID 15183670.
  50. Penin, Xavier; Berge, Christine; Baylac, Michel (May 2002). "Ontogenetic study of the skull in modern humans and the common chimpanzees: neotenic hypothesis reconsidered with a tridimensional Procrustes analysis". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 118 (1): 50–62. doi:10.1002/ajpa.10044. PMID 11953945.
  51. Spicer, J. I.; Burggren, W. W. (2003). "Development of physiological regulatory systems: altering the timing of crucial events". Zoology. 106 (2): 91–99. doi:10.1078/0944-2006-00103. PMID 16351894.

See also

This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.