Hoarding (animal behavior)

Hoarding or caching in animal behavior is the storage of food in locations hidden from the sight of both conspecifics (animals of the same or closely related species) and members of other species.[1] Most commonly, the function of hoarding or caching is to store food in times of surplus for times when food is less plentiful. However, there is evidence that some amount of caching or hoarding is done in order to ripen the food, called ripening caching.[2] The term hoarding is most typically used for rodents, whereas caching is more commonly used in reference to birds, but the behaviors in both animal groups are quite similar.

Western scrub jays cache food such as acorns and insects.

Hoarding is done either on a long-term basis – cached on a seasonal cycle, with food to be consumed months down the line – or on a short term basis, in which case the food will be consumed over a period of one or several days.

Some common animals that cache their food are rodents such as hamsters and squirrels, and many different bird species, such as rooks and woodpeckers. The western scrub jay is noted for its particular skill at caching. There are two types of caching behavior: larder-hoarding, where a species creates a few large caches which it often defends, and scatter-hoarding, where a species will create multiple caches, often with each individual food item stored in a unique place. Both types of caching have their advantage.

Function

Caching behavior is typically a way to save excess edible food for later consumption - either soon to be eaten food, such as when a jaguar hangs a partially eaten prey in a tree to be eaten within a few days, or long term where the food is hidden and retrieved many months later. Caching is a common adaptation to seasonal changes in food availability. In regions where winters are harsh, food availability typically becomes low, and caching food during the times of high food availability in the warmer months provides a significant survival advantage. This phenomenon is referenced in the fable The Ant and the Grasshopper.

However, in ripening caching behavior, animals collect and cache food which is immediately inedible but will become "ripe" and edible after a short while. For instance, tayras (a Central American weasel) have been observed to harvest whole green plantains, hide them, and then come back to eat them after they have ripened.[2] Leafcutter ants harvest pieces of inedible leaves and then cache them in underground chambers to ripen with a fungus which is the main food for the colony. Contrary to popular belief, there is no scientific evidence that crocodilians such as the American Alligator cache large prey underwater to consume later. [3]

Hoard distribution and size

Scatter hoarding is the formation of a large number of small hoards. This behavior is present in both birds (especially the Canada jay) and small mammals, mainly squirrels and other rodents, such as the eastern gray squirrel, fox squirrel, and wood mouse. Specifically, those who do not migrate to warmer climates or hibernate for winter are most likely to scatter hoard.[4][5][6] This behavior plays an important part in seed dispersal, as those seeds that are left uneaten will have a chance to germinate, thus enabling plants to spread their populations effectively.

Cache spacing is the primary technique that scatter hoarders use to protect food from pilferers. By spreading the food supply around geographically, hoarders discourage competitors who happen upon a cache from conducting area-restricted searching for more of the supply. Despite cache spacing, however, hoarders are still unable to completely eliminate the threat of pilferage.[7] However, having multiple cache sites is costly because it requires a good memory. Scatter-hoarders generally have a large hippocampus.[8]

In larder hoarding, the hoard is large and is found in a single place termed a larder, which usually also serves as the nest where the animal lives. Hamsters are famous larder hoarders. Indeed, the German verb "hamstern" (to hoard) is derived from the noun "Hamster" which refers to the rodent; similar verbs are found in various related languages (Dutch hamsteren, and Swedish hamstra). Other languages also draw a clear connection between hamsters and hoarding: Polish chomikować, from chomik – hamster; Hebrew hamster; oger (אוגר) comes from to hoard; le'egor (לאגור). A disadvantage of larder hoarding is that if a cache is raided, this is far more problematic for the animal than if it were a scatter hoarder. While the hoard is much easier to remember the location of, these larger hoards must also be more staunchly defended.

Guarding

Most species are particularly wary of onlooking individuals during caching and ensure that the cache locations are secret.[9][10][11] Not all caches are concealed however, for example shrikes store prey items on thorns on branches in the open.[12]

Shared or individual hoarding

Although a small handful of species share food stores, food hoarding is a solo endeavor for most species, including almost all rodents and birds. For example, a number of jays live in large family groups, but they don't demonstrate sharing of cached food. Rather, they hoard their food supply selfishly, caching and retrieving the supply in secret.[13]

There are only two species in which kin selection has resulted in a shared food store, i.e. beavers (Castor canadensis) and acorn woodpeckers (Melanerpes formicivorous); the former live in family groups and construct winter larders of submerged branches, while the latter are unusual in that they construct a conspicuous communal larder.[14]

Pilfering

Pilferage occurs when one animal takes food from another animal's larder. Some species experience high levels of cache pilferage, up to 30% of the supply per day. Models of scatter hoarding[15][16][17] suggested the value of cached food is equal to the hoarders ability to retrieve it.[18]

Reciprocal pilfering

It has been observed that members of certain species, such as rodents and chickadees, act as both hoarder and pilferer. In other words, pilfering can be reciprocal and, thus, tolerable. Although this kind of food caching system seems cooperative, it has been suggested that it is actually driven by the selfish interests of the individual.[18]

Recaching

Animals recache the food that they've pilfered from other animal's caches. For example, 75% percent of mildly radioactive (thus traceable) Jeffrey pine seeds cached by yellow pine chipmunks were found in two cache sites, 29% of the seeds were found in three sites, 9.4% were found in four sites and 1.3% were found in five sites over a 3-month period.[19] These results, and those from other studies, demonstrate the dynamic nature of the food supplies of scatter hoarding animals.

Deception

Group-foraging common ravens, (Corvus corax), scatter hoard their food and also raid the caches made by others. Cachers withdraw from conspecifics when hiding their food and most often place their caches behind structures, obstructing the view of potential observers. Raiders watch inconspicuously and keep at a distance to cachers close to their cache sites. In response to the presence of potential raiders or because of their initial movements towards caches, the cachers frequently interrupt caching, change cache sites, or recover their food items. These behaviors suggest that ravens are capable of withholding information about their intentions, which may qualify as tactical deception.[20]

Similarly, Eurasian jays (Garrulus glandarius) when being watched by another jay, prefer to cache food behind an opaque barrier rather than a transparent barrier, suggesting they may opt to cache in out-of-view locations to reduce the likelihood of other jays pilfering their caches.[21]

gollark: "Free speech" the legal thing is that, "free speech" the general concept less so.
gollark: > free speech is the right to not get arrested for what you say. your free speech isnt violated when people start not liking you for what you say or simply kick you out of their communities for it.That's not *exactly* right.
gollark: People who did not agree on Gibson were wrong.
gollark: We agreed on gibson because he was not lyricly.
gollark: Not necessarily.

See also

References

  1. Vander Wall, Stephen B. (1990) Food Hoarding in Animals. University of Chicago Press. ISBN 0-226-84735-7
  2. Soley, F. G.; Alvarado-Díaz, I. (2011). "Prospective thinking in a mustelid? Eira barbara (Carnivora) cache unripe fruits to consume them once ripened". Naturwissenschaften. 98 (8): 693–698. Bibcode:2011NW.....98..693S. doi:10.1007/s00114-011-0821-0. ISSN 0028-1042. PMID 21739130.
  3. Moore, Clifford B. (1954). "The Grinning Crocodilian and His Folklore". The Scientific Monthly. 78 (4): 225–231. Bibcode:1954SciMo..78..225M. ISSN 0096-3771. JSTOR 21395.
  4. Waite, Thomas A. (1992). "Social Hoarding and a Load Size-Distance Relationship in Gray Jays". Condor. 94 (4): 995–998. doi:10.2307/1369297. JSTOR 1369297. Retrieved 21 November 2016.
  5. Jokinen, S; Suhonen, J (1995). "Food Caching By Willow and Crested Tits: A Test of Scatterhoarding Models". Ecology. 76 (3): 892–898. doi:10.2307/1939354. JSTOR 1939354.
  6. Thorington, R.W., Jr., Koprowski, J.L., Steele, M.A., and Whatton, J. (2012) Squirrels of the World. Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, 472 pp.
  7. Hampton, R. R. & Sherry, D. F. (1994) The effects of cache loss on choice of cache sites in the black-capped chickadee. Behav. Ecol. 5:44-50
  8. Dally, Joanna M.; Emery, Nathan J.; Clayton, Nicola S. (2005). "Cache protection strategies by western scrub-jays, Aphelocoma californica: implications for social cognition". Animal Behaviour. 70 (6): 1251–1263. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.183.6317. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.02.009.
  9. Dally, Joanna M.; Emery, Nathan J.; Clayton, Nicola S. (2006). "Food-Caching Western Scrub-Jays Keep Track of Who Was Watching When". Science. 312 (5780): 1662–1665. Bibcode:2006Sci...312.1662D. doi:10.1126/science.1126539. PMID 16709747.
  10. Heinrich, B. & Pepper, J. W. (1998) Influence of competitors on caching behavior in the common raven, Corvus corax. Anim. Behav. 56:1083-1090
  11. Emery, N. J.; Clayton, N. S. (2001). "Effects of experience and social context on prospective caching strategies by scrub jays". Nature. 414 (6862): 443–446. Bibcode:2001Natur.414..443E. doi:10.1038/35106560. PMID 11719804.
  12. Yosef, Reuven (2008), "Family Laniidae (Shrikes)", in Josep, del Hoyo; Andrew, Elliott; David, Christie (eds.), Handbook of the Birds of the World. Volume 13, Penduline-tits to Shrikes, Barcelona: Lynx Edicions, pp. 732–773, ISBN 978-84-96553-45-3
  13. Waite, T.A., (1992). Gray jay scatterhoarding behavior, rate maximization and the effect of local cache density. Ornis. Scand., 23: 175-182
  14. Koenig, W.D. and Mumme, R.L., (1987). Population Ecology of the Cooperatively Breeding Acorn Woodpecker. Princeton, New Jersey. Princeton University Press. 0691084645
  15. Stapanian, M.A.; Smith, C.C. (1978). "A model for seed scatterhoarding: coevolution of fox squirrels and black walnuts". Ecology. 59 (5): 884–896. doi:10.2307/1938541. JSTOR 1938541.
  16. Stapanian, M.A.; Smith, C.C. (1984). "Density-dependent survival of scatterhoarded nuts: an experimental approach". Ecology. 65 (5): 1387–1396. doi:10.2307/1939119. JSTOR 1939119.
  17. Clarkson, K.; Eden, S.F.; Sutherland, W.J.; Houston, A.I. (1986). "Density, dependence and magpie hoarding". J. Anim. Ecol. 55 (1): 111–121. doi:10.2307/4696. JSTOR 4696.
  18. Vander Wall, S.B.; Jenkins, S.H. (2003). "Reciprocal pilferage and the evolution of food-hoarding behavior". Behavioral Ecology. 14 (5): 656–667. doi:10.1093/beheco/arg064.
  19. Vander Wall, S.B.; Joyner, J.W. (1998). "Recaching of Jeffrey pine (Pinus jeffreyi) seeds by yellow pine chipmunks (Tamias amoenus): potential effects on plant reproductive success". Can. J. Zool. 76: 154–162. doi:10.1139/cjz-76-1-154.
  20. Bugnyarf, T.; Kotrschal, K. (2002). "Observational learning and the raiding of food caches in ravens, Corvus corax: is it 'tactical' deception?". Animal Behaviour. 64 (2): 185–195. doi:10.1006/anbe.2002.3056.
  21. Legg, E.W.; Clayton, N.S. (2014). "Eurasian jays (Garrulus glandarius) conceal caches from onlookers". Animal Cognition. 17 (5): 1223–1226. doi:10.1007/s10071-014-0743-2. PMC 4138428. PMID 24638877.

Further reading

Jenkins, Stewart H. and Breck, Stewart W. (1998) Differences in food hoarding among six species of heteromyid rodents. J Mammal. 79:1221-1233.

This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.