Social competence
Social competence consists of social, emotional, cognitive and behavioral skills needed for successful social adaptation. Social competence also reflects having an ability to take another's perspective concerning a situation, learn from past experiences, and apply that learning to the changes in social interactions.[1]
Social competence is the foundation upon which expectations for future interaction with others is built, and upon which individuals develop perceptions of their own behavior. Social competence frequently encompasses social skills, social communication, and interpersonal communication.[1]
History
Past and current research intends to further the understanding of how and why social competence is important in healthy social development. The study of social competence began in the early 20th century. A noteworthy discovery was that social competence was related to future mental health, thus fueling research on how children interact with their peers and function in social situations.[2] As research developed, different definitions and measurement techniques developed to suit these new findings.
In the 1930s, researchers began investigating peer groups and how children's characteristics affected their positions within these peer groups. In the 1950s and 1960s, research established that children's social competence was related to future mental health (such as maladaptive outcomes in adulthood), as well as problems in school settings.[2] Research on social competence expanded greatly from this point on, as increasing amounts of evidence demonstrated the importance of social interactions.[3]
Mid-century, researchers began to view social competence in terms of problem-solving skills and strategies in social situations. Social competence was now conceptualized in terms of effective social functioning and information processing. In the 1970s and 1980s, research began focusing on the impact of children's behavior on relationships,[2] which influenced the study of the effectiveness of teaching children social skills that are age, gender, and context specific.
In an effort to determine why some children were not exhibiting social skills in some interactions, many researchers devised social information processing models to explain what happens in a social interaction.[2] These models concentrated on factors in interactions such as behavior, how people process and judge each other, and how they process social cues. They also focus on how people select social goals, decide on the best response to a situation and enacting the chosen response. Studies such as this often looked at the relationship between social cognition and social competence.[2]
A prominent researcher of social competence in the mid-1980s was Frank Gresham. He identified three sub-domains of social competence: adaptive behavior, social skills, and peer acceptance (peer acceptance is often used to assess social competence). Research during this time often focused on children who were not displaying social skills in efforts to identify and help these children who were potentially at risk of long-term negative outcomes due to poor social interactions.[2] Gresham proposed that these children could have one of four deficits: skill deficits, in which children did not have the knowledge or cognitive abilities to carry out a certain behavior, performance deficits, self-control skill deficits, and self-control performance deficits, in which children had excessive anxiety or impulsivity that prohibited proper execution of the behaviors or skills they knew and understood.
Despite all the developments and changes in the conceptualization of social competence throughout the 20th century, there was still a general lack of agreement about the definition and measurement of social competence during the 1980s.[4] The definitions of the 1980s were less ambiguous than previous definitions, but they often did not acknowledge the age, situation, and skill specificity implicit in the complex construct of social competence.[5]
Approaches and theories
Peer regard/status approaches
These approaches define social competence based on how popular one is with his peers.[6] The more well-liked one is, the more socially competent they are.[7]
Social skill approaches
These approaches use behaviors as a guideline.[6] Behaviors that demonstrate social skills are compiled and are collectively identified as social competence.[7][8]
Relationship approaches
According to these approaches, social competence is assessed by the quality of one's relationships and the ability to form relationships. Competence depends on the skills of both members of the relationship; a child may appear more socially competent if interacting with a socially skilled partner.[7] Commentators on some online incel communities have advocated government programs wherein socially awkward men are helped or women are incentivized to go on dates with them.[9]
Functional approaches
The functional approach is context-specific and concerned with the identification of social goals and tasks. This approach also focuses on the outcomes of social behavior and the processes leading to those outcomes. Information-processing models of social skills are important here, and based on the idea that social competence results from social-cognitive processes.[7]
Models
Early models of social competence stress the role of context and situation specificity in operationalizing the competence construct.[10] These models also allow for the organization and integration of the various component skills, behaviors and cognitions associated with social competence. Whereas global definitions focus on the "ends" rather than the "means" by which such ends are achieved,[10] a number of models directly attend to the theorized processes underlying competence.[10][11][12] These process models are context specific and seek to identify critical social goals and tasks associated with social competence. Other models focus on the often overlooked distinction between social competence and the indices (i.e., skills and abilities) used to gauge it.
Behavioral–analytic model
Goldfried and D'Zurilla[10] developed a five-step behavioral-analytic model outlining a definition of social competence.
The specific steps proposed in the model include: (1) situational analysis, (2) response enumeration, (3) response evaluation, (4) measure development, and (5) evaluation of the measure.
- Situation analysis – a critical situation is defined on the basis of certain criteria, which include:
- occurs with some frequency
- presents a difficult response decision
- results in a range of possible responses in a given population.[10] Situation identification and analysis is accomplished through a variety of methods, including direct observation by self or others, interviews, and surveys.
- Response enumeration – sampling of possible responses to each situation is obtained. Procedures for generating response alternatives include direct observation, role plays, and simulations in video and/or written formats.
- Response evaluation – the enumerated responses are judged for effectiveness by "significant others" in the environment. An important element is that a consensus must emerge or the particular item is removed from future consideration.
In the last two steps (4 and 5) a measure for assessing social competence is developed and evaluated.
Social information-processing model
A social information-processing model is a widely used means for understanding social competence.[11] The social information-processing model focuses more directly on the cognitive processes underlying response selection, enactment, and evaluation. Using a computer metaphor, the reformulated social information-processing model outlines a six-step nonlinear process with various feedback loops linking children's social cognition and behavior. Difficulties that arise at any of the steps generally translates into social competence deficits.
The six steps are:
- Observation and encoding of relevant stimuli – attending to and encoding non-verbal and verbal social cues, both external and internal.
- Interpretation and mental representation of cues – understanding what has happened during the social encounter, as well as the cause and intent underlying the interaction.
- Clarification of goals – determining what one's objective is for the interaction and how to put forth an understanding of those goals.
- Representation of situation is developed by accessing long-term memory or construction – the interaction is compared to previous situations stored in long-term memory and the previous outcomes of those interactions.
- Response decision/selection
- Behavioral enactment and evaluation
Tri-component model
Another way to conceptualize social competence is to consider three underlying subcomponents in a hierarchical framework.[13]
- Social Adjustment
- Social Performance
- Social Skills
The top of the hierarchy includes the most advanced level, social adjustment. Social adjustment is defined as the extent to which an individual achieves society's developmentally appropriate goals.[13] The goals are conceived of as different "statuses" to be achieved by members of a society (e.g., health, legal, academic or occupational, socioeconomic, social, emotional, familial, and relational statuses). The next level is social performance – or the degree to which an individual's responses to relevant social situations meet socially valid criteria. The lowest level of the hierarchy is social skills, which are defined as specific abilities (i.e. overt behavior, social cognitive skills, and emotional regulation) allowing for the competent performance within social tasks.
The quadripartite model
The essential core elements of competence are theorized to consist of four superordinate sets of skills, abilities, and capacities: (1) cognitive skills and abilities, (2) behavioral skills, (3) emotional competencies, and (4) motivational and expectancy sets.[12]
- Cognitive skills and abilities – cultural and social knowledge necessary for effective functioning in society (i.e., academic and occupational skills and abilities, decision-making ability, and the processing of information)
- Behavioral skills – knowledge of behavioral responses and the ability to enact them (i.e., negotiation, role- or perspective-taking, assertiveness, conversational skills, and prosocial skills)
- Emotional skills – affect regulation and affective capacities for facilitating socially competent responding and forming relationships
- Motivational and expectancy sets – an individual's value structure, moral development, and sense of efficacy and control.
The developmental framework
Social competence develops over time, and the mastery of social skills and interpersonal social interactions emerge at various time points on the developmental continuum (infancy to adolescence) and build on previously learned skills and knowledge.[1] Key facets and markers of social competence that are remarkably consistent across the developmental periods (early childhood, middle/late childhood, adolescence) include prosocial skills (i.e., friendly, cooperative, helpful behaviors) and self-control or regulatory skills (i.e., anger management, negotiation skills, problem-solving skills).[1] However, as developmental changes occur in the structure and quality of interactions, as well as in cognitive and language abilities, these changes affect the complexity of skills and behaviors contributing to socially competent responding.[14]
Contributing factors
Temperament
Temperament is a construct that describes a person's biological response to the environment.[1] Issues such as soothability, rhythmicity, sociability, and arousal make up this construct.[1] Most often sociability contributes to the development of social competence.
Attachment
Social experiences rest on the foundation of parent–child relationships, and are important in the later development of social skills and behaviors. Attachment of an infant to a care-giver is important for the development of later social skills and behaviors[15] that develop social competence. Attachment helps the infant learn that the world is predictable and trustworthy or in other instances capricious and cruel. Ainsworth[15] describes four types of attachment styles in infancy, including secure, anxious–avoidant, anxious–resistant and disorganized/disoriented. The foundation of the attachment bond allows the child to venture out from his/her mother to try new experiences and new interactions. Children with secure attachment styles tend to show higher levels of social competence relative to children with unsecure attachment, including anxious–avoidant, anxious–resistant, and disorganized/disoriented.[1]
Parenting style
Parents are the primary source of social and emotional development in infancy, early, and middle/late childhood. The socialization practices of parents influence whether their child will develop social competence. Parenting style captures two important elements of parenting: parental warmth/responsiveness and parental control/demandingness.[16] Parental responsiveness (warmth or supportiveness) refers to "the extent to which parents intentionally foster individuality, self-regulation, and self-assertion by being attuned, supportive, and acquiescent to children's special needs and demands."[17] Parental demandingness (behavioral control) refers to "the claims parents make on children to become integrated into the family whole, by their maturity demands, supervision, disciplinary efforts and willingness to confront the child who disobeys."[17] Categorizing parents according to whether they are high or low on parental demandingness and responsiveness creates a typology of four parenting styles: indulgent/permissive, authoritarian, authoritative, and indifferent/uninvolved. Each of these parenting styles reflects patterns of parental values, practices, and behaviors[17] and a distinct balance of responsiveness and demandingness.
Parenting style contributes to child well-being in the domains of social competence, academic performance, psychosocial development, and problem behavior.[17] Research based on parent interviews, child reports, and parent observations consistently find that:
- Children and adolescents whose parents are authoritative rate themselves and are rated by objective measures as more socially and instrumentally competent than those whose parents are nonauthoritative.[17][18][19]
- Children and adolescents whose parents are uninvolved perform most poorly in all domains.
Other factors that contribute to social competence include teacher relationships, peer groups, neighborhood, and community.[20]
Related problem behaviors
An important researcher in the study of social competence, Voeller, states that there are three clusters of problem behaviors that lead to the impairment of social competence.[21] Voeller clusters include: (1) an aggressive and hostile group, (2) a perceptual deficits subgroup, and (3) a group with difficulties in self-regulation.
- Children with aggressive and hostile behaviors are those whose acting out behaviors negatively influence their ability to form relationships, and sustain interpersonal interactions.[21] Aggressive and hostile children tend to have deficiencies in social information-processing, and employ inappropriate social problem solving strategies to social situations.[21] They also tend to search for fewer facts in a social situation and pay more attention to the aggressive social interactions presented in an interaction.[11]
- Children with perceptual deficits do not perceive the environment appropriately and interpret interpersonal interactions inaccurately. They also have difficulty reading social cues, facial expressions and body gestures.
- Children with self-regulation deficits tend to have the classic difficulties in executive functions.
Assessments
While understanding the components of social competence continue to be empirically validated, the assessment of social competence is not well-studied[8] and continues to develop in procedures.[1] There are a variety of methods for the assessment of social competence and often include one (or more) of the following:
- Child–adolescent interview
- Observations
- Parent report measures
- Self-report measures
- Sociometric measures (i.e., peer nominations)
- Teachers report measures
Interventions
Following the increased awareness of the importance of social competence in childhood, interventions are used to help children with social difficulties. Historically, intervention efforts did not improve children's peer status or yield long-lasting effects. Interventions did not take into account that social competence problems do not occur in isolation, but alongside other problems as well. Thus, current intervention efforts target social competence both directly and indirectly in varying contexts.
Preschool and early-childhood interventions
Early childhood interventions targeting social skills directly improve the peer relations of children. These interventions focus on at-risk groups such as single, adolescent mothers and families of children with early behavior problems. Interventions targeting both children and families have the highest success rates. When children reach preschool age, social competence interventions focus on the preschool context and teach prosocial skills. Such interventions generally entail teaching problem-solving and conflict management skills, sharing, and improving parenting skills. Interventions improve children's social competence and interactions with peers in the short-term and they also reduce long-term risk, such as substance abuse or delinquent behavior.
School-age interventions
Social competence becomes more complicated as children grow older, and most intervention efforts for this age group target individual skills, the family, and the classroom setting. These programs focus on training skills in problem solving, emotional understanding, cooperation, and self-control. Understanding one's emotions, and the ability to communicate these emotions, is strongly emphasized. The most effective programs give children the opportunity to practice the new skills that they learn. Results of social competence interventions include decreased aggression, improved self-control, and increased conflict resolution skills.
See also
References
- Semrud-Clikeman, M. (2007). Social competence in children. New York, NY: Springer Science+Business Media.
- .
- O'Malley, J. Michael (1977). "Research Perspective on Social Competence". Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development. 23 (1): 29–44. ISSN 0026-0150. JSTOR 23083895.
- Dodge, K.A.; Asher, S.R. & Parkhurst, J.T. (1989). C. Ames & R. Ames (ed.). Research on Motivation in Education. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, Inc.CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
- Pellegrini, A.D. (2000). The Child at School: Interaction with Peers and Teachers. London: Arnold.
- Stump, K.N.; Ratliff, J.M.; Wu, Y.P.; Hawley, P.H. (2009). Matson, J.L. (ed.). Social competence. New York, NY, US: Springer Science + Business Media; US. pp. 23–37.CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
- Rose-Krasnor, L. (1997). "The nature of social competence: A theoretical review". Social Development. 6: 111–35. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.1997.tb00097.x.
- .
- http://www.huffingtonpost.co.uk/entry/socially-anxiety-dating-government-should-pay-women-date-men_n_3293626
- Goldfried, M.R.; D'Zurilla, T.J. (1969). C. Spielberger (ed.). Current topics in clinical and community psychology. New York: Academic. pp. 151–96.
- Crick, N.R.; Dodge, K.A. (1994). "A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children's social adjustment". Psychological Bulletin. 115: 74–101. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74.
- Felner, R.D.; Lease, A.M. & Phillips, R.S.C. (1990). T.P. Gullota, G.R. Adams & R. Montemayor (ed.). The development of social competence in adolescence. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. pp. 245–64.CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
- Cavell, T.A. (1990). "Social adjustment, social performance, and social skills: A tri-component model of social competence". Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 19 (2): 111–22. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp1902_2.
- "Social Competence - Importance of social competence, Social competence deficits and peer rejection, Developmental changes and social competence". psychology.jrank.org. Retrieved 2019-03-06.
- Ainsworth, M. (1979). "Infant-mother attachment". American Psychologist. 34 (10): 932–7. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.34.10.932. PMID 517843.
- Maccoby, E.E.; Martin, J.A. (1983). P.H. Mussen & E.M. Hetherington (ed.). Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social development (4th ed.). New York: Wiley. pp. 1–101.
- Baumrind, D. (1991). "The influence of parenting style on adolescent competence and substance use". Journal of Early Adolescence. 11: 56–95. doi:10.1177/0272431691111004.
- Weiss, L.H.; Schwarz, J.C. (1996). "The relationship between parenting types and older adolescents' personality, academic achievement, adjustment, and substance use". Child Development. 67 (5): 2, 101–14. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01846.x.
- Miller, N.B.; Cowan, P.A.; Cowan, C.P. & Hetherington, E.M. (1993). "Externalizing in preschoolers and early adolescents: A cross-study replication of a family model". Developmental Psychology. 29: 3–18. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.29.1.3.CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
- Criss, Michael M.; Shaw, Daniel S.; Moilanen, Kristin L.; Hitchings, Julia E.; Ingoldsby, Erin M. (2009-08-01). "Family, Neighborhood, and Peer Characteristics as Predictors of Child Adjustment: A Longitudinal Analysis of Additive and Mediation Models". Social Development. 18 (3): 511–535. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00520.x. ISSN 0961-205X. PMC 2712303. PMID 20161200.
- Voeller, K.K.S. (1994). G.R. Lyon (ed.). Frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company. pp. 523–54.