Pack (canine)

Pack is a social group of conspecific canids. Not all species of canids form packs; for example, small canids like the red fox do not. Pack size and social behaviour within packs varies across species.

Wolf packs often work cooperatively, as in this bison hunt at Yellowstone National Park.
A small pack of coyotes in Yellowstone National Park
A hunting pack of African wild dogs
Pack size distribution of the African wild dog.

Pack behavior in specific species

African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus) live and hunt in packs. Males assist in raising the pups, and remain with their pack for life, while the females leave their birth pack at about the age of two and a half years old to join a pack with no females. Males outnumber the females in a pack, and usually only one female breeds with all of the males. African wild dogs are not territorial, and they hunt cooperatively in their packs, running down large game and tearing it apart. They cooperate in caring for wounded and sick pack members as well as the young.[1]

Gray wolves (Canis lupus) usually live in packs which consist of the adult parents and their offspring of perhaps the last 2 or 3 years. The adult parents are usually unrelated and other unrelated wolves may sometimes join the pack.[2] Wolves usually hunt in packs, but they hunt alone in spring and summer months when there is plenty of prey available. They are found in both Eurasia and North America.

Black-backed jackals (Canis mesomelas) in Southern and Eastern Africa and coyotes (Canis latrans) in North America have a single long-term mate, but usually hunt alone or in pairs.[3][4] Both parents care for the young, and the parents and their current offspring are the pack. They occasionally cooperate in larger packs to hunt large game.

The Ethiopian wolf (Canis simensis) pack members hunt for rodents alone and come together mainly to defend their territory from rival packs.[5]

Pack behavior in grey wolves

All individuals benefit from being a member of the wolf pack; the weak are supported by the efforts of stronger wolves,[6] and higher ranking individuals enjoy better and larger kills than could be taken on their own.[7] Protection is granted by sheer number, and larger, more plentiful territory can be won and sustained.[8]. Care and protection of young is shared, and knowledge can be passed down through generations, creating a unique culture within each group[6]

The pack is typically a nuclear family unit.[9] It often consists of 5-10 (though in areas of high prey abundance can be up to 20)[10] mostly related individuals, specifically consisting of a typically unrelated breeding pair also known as the alphas,[11] their offspring, and occasionally a handful of other wolves which can be related or not.[9] Membership may be fluid and is subject to change.[12] Outside wolves may be shunned or, more rarely, accepted, depending on the specific circumstances. Genetic variability can become limited within such an interrelated group, and so conditions for gene flow must exist.[11] Outside wolves can provide these opportunities.[6] A pack may be accepting of another wolf into their group if it is a distant relative,[9] if reproduction rates are low due to loss or infertility of an alpha, or if their numbers are significantly reduced.[11]

Characterization of wolves into dominance hierarchies of alpha, beta, and omega were based on behavioral studies of unrelated wolves in captivity, and this assemblage largely does not apply to natural wolf packs, which are familial units.[9]

The lone wolf

These singular outside wolves, often referred to as lone wolves, are vulnerable to food scarcity and territorial attacks, and generally make up less than 15% of a wolf population. Lone wolves usually result from sexually mature offspring leaving their parental pack, though may also occur if harassed subordinates chose to disperse. In times of prey scarcity, low ranking wolves may choose to go off on their own if the pack cannot supply sufficient food. These lone wolves may then attempt to join into an existing wolf pack or, more commonly, find a mate and begin a new pack family as the alphas.[11]

The alphas (breeding pair)

Within the wolf pack, the alphas, now more commonly referred to as the breeding pair, are the only wolves in the group which breed and produce offspring; they are the matriarch and patriarch of the family.[10] It was previously believed to be common for an aging or sick alpha to be replaced by one of their offspring,[11] but more recent studies have shown this incestuous behavior to be very rare.[13]

The pups

Importance of the alpha is rivaled only by that of the pups.[14] The fundamental purpose of the pack is the successful production of offspring, and so raising of the litter is a collaborative venture – all members contribute to their development.[6] In times of scarcity, the breeding pair will often prioritize care of the pups, and preferentially feed the youngest wolves first.[9] Despite this committed involvement, pup mortality is high, with researchers citing that only roughly 30% survive their first year of life.[15] Those who survive, however, grow up with the added advantage of being surrounded by numerous care-takers and teachers. There exists a culture within wolf packs, and this is passed on to the offspring by the elders of the group.  Pups learn something from each member of the pack, and attain the vital social skills required to create those powerful bonds upon which wolf societal structure relies.[6]

White huskies dog sledding

Dominance and the alpha wolf

Animals which typically predominate over others are associated with the term alpha. Among pack-living wolves, alpha wolves are the genetic parents of most cubs in the pack. Such access to mating females creates strong selective pressure for intra-sex competition.[16]

Wolves show deference to the alpha pair in their pack by allowing them to allocate the distribution of food, typically preferentially feeding the youngest wolves. Wolves use eye contact and posture as an indicator of dominance or submission, which are largely age-based; these postures are rare except in relation to food, as described previously. The smaller and more nuclear a pack is, the status of alpha is less likely to be obtained through fighting, and young wolves instead leave the pack to find a mate and produce offspring of their own.[17] Larger or less-nuclear packs may operate differently and possess more complex and flexible social structures.[18]

In the case of other wild canids, the alpha male may not have exclusive access to the alpha female;[19] moreover, other pack members may guard the maternity den used by the alpha female; as with the African wild dog, Lycaon pictus.[20]

As dominant roles may be deemed normal among social species with extended parenting, it has been suggested that the additional term alpha is not required merely to describe dominance due to its ubiquity, but should be reserved for where they are the predominant pack progenitor. For instance, wolf biologist L. David Mech stated

"calling a wolf an alpha is usually no more appropriate than referring to a human parent or a doe deer as an alpha. Any parent is dominant to its young offspring, so alpha adds no information. Why not refer to an alpha female as the female parent, the breeding female, the matriarch, or simply the mother? Such a designation emphasizes not the animal's dominant status, which is trivial information, but its role as pack progenitor, which is critical information. The one use we may still want to reserve for alpha is in the relatively few large wolf packs comprised of multiple litters. ... In such cases the older breeders are probably dominant to the younger breeders and perhaps can more appropriately be called the alphas. ... The point here is not so much the terminology but what the terminology falsely implies: a rigid, force-based dominance hierarchy."[21]

Use in dog training

One of the most persistent theories in dog training literature is the idea of the alpha wolf, an individual gray wolf who uses body language and, when needed, physical force to maintain its dominance within the wolf pack. The idea was first reported in early wolf research.[2] It was subsequently adopted by dog trainers.[22] The term alpha was popularized as early as 1976 in the dog training book How to Be Your Dog's Best Friend (Monks of New Skete), which introduced the idea of the alpha roll, a technique for punishing unwanted dog behaviours. Psychologist and dog trainer Stanley Coren in the 2001 book How to Speak Dog says "you are the alpha dog...You must communicate that you are the pack leader and dominant".[23]

Training techniques assumed to be wolf pack related such as scruff shaking, the alpha roll and recommendations to be alpha to the dog continue to be used and recommended by some dog training instructors.[24] It has been suggested that the use of such techniques may have more to do with human psychology than with dog behaviour; "dominance hierarchies and dominance disputes and testing are a fundamental characteristic of all social groups... But perhaps only we humans learn to use punishment primarily to gain for ourselves the reward of being dominant.[25] Most leading veterinary and animal behavior associations, and most contemporary trainers would agree, advocating the use of rewards to teach commands and encourage good communication between owners and their pets. Modern best practices dictate an abandonment of outdated "pack" methods.[26] Some canine behaviourists suggest that kind, efficient training uses games to teach commands which can be utilised to benefit the owner's everyday life.[27]

gollark: Wait, I have those permissions, and *LyricLy* frequently pings everyone and is unpunished for it.
gollark: "Lyrical Tyrant" or something.
gollark: What?
gollark: I vote for @⁡everyone!
gollark: I DID vote for gibson and my VOTE IS FOR gibson.

See also

References

  1. Animal Diversity Web. "Lycaon pictus: Information". University of Michigan. Retrieved 21 April 2008.
  2. L. David Mech. "Schenkel's Classic Wolf Behavior Study Available in English". Archived from the original on 10 August 2013. Retrieved 21 April 2008. ...Schenkel’s 1947 “Expressions Studies on Wolves”, the study that gave rise to the now outmoded notion of alpha wolves. That concept was based on the old idea that wolves fight within a pack to gain dominance and that the winner is the “alpha” wolf. Today we understand that most wolf packs consist of a pair of adults called “parents” or “breeders,” and their offspring.
  3. Animal Diversity Web. "Canis mesomelas: Information". University of Michigan. Retrieved 21 April 2008.
  4. "Coyotes 101 – CoyoteSmart". www.coyotesmarts.org. Retrieved 14 April 2018.
  5. "Ethiopian Wolf". Animal Info. 2005-03-07. Retrieved 2015-02-26.
  6. 1.      Living With Wolves. The Social Wolf. Retrieved February 20, 2019, from https://www.livingwithwolves.org/about-wolves/social-wolf/
  7. 1.      Schmidt, P., & Mech, D. (1997). Wolf Pack Size And Food Acquisition. Am Nat, 150(4), 513-517. doi:10.1086/286079
  8. 1.      Pack Power. (2015, March 30). The Economist. Retrieved March 2, 2019, from https://www.economist.com/science-and-technology/2015/05/30/pack-power
  9. 1.      Mech, D. (1999). Alpha Status, Dominance, and Division of Labor in Wolf Packs. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 77, 1196-1203. Retrieved March 17, 2019, from https://www.wolf.org/wp-content/uploads/2013/09/267alphastatus_english.pdf.
  10. 1.      Western Wildlife Outreach. Wolf Ecology and Behavior. Retrieved February 24, 2019, from http://westernwildlife.org/gray-wolf-outreach-project/biology-behavior-4/
  11. 1.      Tamonga Nature Park. (n.d.). Wolf Pack Society. Retrieved February 24, 2019, from http://www.tamonganaturepark.com/wolf-pack-society.html
  12. 1.      International Wolf Center. (2014, January). Wolf FAQ's. Retrieved March 2, 2019, from https://www.wolf.org/wolf-info/basic-wolf-info/wolf-faqs/#g
  13. Smith, Deborah; Meier, Thomas; Geffen, Eli; Mech, L. David; et al. (1 July 1997). "Is incest common in gray wolf packs?". Behavioral Ecology. 8 (4): 384–391. doi:10.1093/beheco/8.4.384.
  14. 1.      Ellis, S., & Junor, P. (2009). The Man Who Lives With Wolves (1st ed.,). New York, NY: Harmony Books.
  15. 1.      Smith, D. (2015, March 5). Wolf pup survival a fragile thing. StarTribune. Retrieved March 27, 2019, from http://www.startribune.com/wolf-pup-survival-a-fragile-thing/295226071/
  16. Sillero-Zubiri, Gottelli, MacDonald (1996). "Male Philopatry, Extra-Pack Copulations and Inbreeding Avoidance in Ethiopian Wolves (Canis simensis)". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 38: 331–340. doi:10.1007/s002650050249 via JSTOR.CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  17. Mech, L. David. 1999.
  18. Dutcher, Jim and Jamie. Wolves At Our Door, Simon and Schuster, 2002
  19. Gary Greenberg and Maury M. Haraway. 1998
  20. C. Michael Hogan. 2009
  21. Mech, L. David (1999). "Alpha Status, Dominance, and Division of Labor in Wolf Packs". Jamestown, ND: Northern Prairie Wildlife Research Center Online. Canadian Journal of Zoology 77:1196-1203. Archived from the original on 2004-12-21. Retrieved 21 April 2008. The point here is not so much the terminology but what the terminology falsely implies: a rigid, force-based dominance hierarchy.
  22. "ClickerSolutions Training Articles - The History and Misconceptions of Dominance Theory". Clickersolutions.com. Retrieved 2015-02-26.
  23. Coren, Stanley (2001). "20". How to Speak Dog (First Fireside ed.). Simon & Schuster. p. 250. ISBN 0-684-86534-3.
  24. "BC Boards". Bordercollie.org. Retrieved 2015-02-26.
  25. Pryor, Karen (August 1999). "4". Don't Shoot the Dog! (Bantam trade paperback ed.). Bantam Books. p. 108. ISBN 0-553-38039-7.
  26. "Position Statement on the Use of Dominance Theory in Behavior Modification of Animals" (PDF). American Veterinary Society of Animal Behavior. Retrieved 22 July 2017.
  27. "Dog behaviour pack leaders debunked". Affinity Dog Training. Retrieved 2013-08-16.
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.