Goal theory

Goal theory is the label used in educational psychology to discuss research into motivation to learn. Goals of learning are thought to be a key factor influencing the level of a student's intrinsic motivation.

Main axes

Research in goal theory has identified the following dichotomies:

Task/ego involvement

A student is described as task-involved when they are interested in the task for its own qualities. This is associated with higher intrinsic motivation. Task-involved students are less threatened by failure because their own ego is not tied up in the success of the task.[1]

A student who is ego-involved will be seeking to perform the task to boost their own ego, for the praise that completing the task might attract, or because completing the task confirms their own self-concept (e.g. clever, strong, funny etc...). Ego-involved students can become very anxious or discouraged in the face of failure, because such failure challenges their self-concept.

Approach/avoidance goals

Not all goals are directed towards approaching a desirable outcome (e.g., demonstrating competence). Goals can also be directed towards avoiding an undesirable outcome (e.g., avoiding the demonstration of incompetence to others).[2]

It is thought that approach goals contribute positively to intrinsic motivation whereas avoidance goals do not.

Performance goals

A performance goal is a goal focused on gaining favorable judgement or avoiding unfavorable judgements by others. Performance goals focuses on ensuring that one's performance is noticeably superior to others. This motivation to outperform others is what enables the person to strive for more achievement in and outside of school and work as well.

In the classroom

Performance goals can heavily impact adolescents in the classroom. This deep desire to out-do those around oneself can alter classroom ideologies in each student; some for the better and sometimes for the worse. For the betterment of performance in class, performance goals lead students to place a greater importance on GPA and class rankings. This in turn, leads to better academic performance. Along with a focus on grades, students see exams as a competitive competition that also allows them to enhance their performance.[3]

There is a significant advantage in academic performance in students who possess performance goals in the classroom. It also generates a healthy form of competition between peers enhancing peer relationships and grades among all of those particular students.[4]

Performance goals lead to a strong sense of commitment that can appear in the classroom and also outside of the classroom as well. The student exemplifies a strong relationship with the goal of doing better than others, and this leads to a longterm commitment to achieving that goal.[5] On the other hand, there can be extensive downsides that can come along with focusing entirely on out-performing others. Students can feel accomplished when they receive a better grade on the test than everyone else, but that can simply be linked to memorization and not full comprehension.[4] There comes a conflict when the student attempts to fully comprehend new information on top of trying to focus on doing better than those around them. The student may not be able to handle the combined pressure of learning and constant competition; one of those two variables must alter. Other concerns involve stress on the student to try to keep up with those around them, tension in the classroom as a student struggles with asking questions for fear of seeming incapable to others, and anxiety and frustration with all of those variables on top of each other.[5]

Other developments

Other researchers have adopted a more complex perspective on goals, arguing that there are many different kinds of goals individuals can have in achievement settings. For instance, Ford and Nichols (1987) extended this point of view into within-person goals and person-environment goals, which lays equal significance on learners per se and learning environment.[6]

Nevertheless, all the theories are devoted to studying the types of goals as well as their impact on multiple facets of learning. In other words, research that takes goals as a dependent variable remains scarce. Such a strategy to take goals for granted could be defended on the grounds that one cannot deal with all aspects of so complex an issue and that the theorists possibly feel the question of how goals originate was not relevant to the models they developed.

On the other hand, young children are frequently ignored within this area, based on the assumption that they might not have a clear pattern of setting a goal or they even do not own a goal when starting a task. Klahr argued that although there are large adult-child differences in overall problem-solving performance, even preschoolers have rudimentary forms of strategies such as means-ends analysis that rely on the use of goals.[7] Thus, expanding the subject selection range and focusing on the process of goal-setting are expected to be the two main tasks in future research direction.

gollark: I have ominous "oh bee a thing is behind me" feelings sometimes. However, this is not actually *true*.
gollark: It's evidence, sure, but weak.
gollark: The feeling of something is very different to a thing, though.
gollark: Did you know? An invisible swarm of bees is behind you.
gollark: Well, you could do one *over* candles, in parallel.

References

  1. Nicholls, John G.; Cobb, Paul; Wood, Terry; Yackel, Erna; Patashnick, Michael (1990-01-01). "Assessing Students' Theories of Success in Mathematics: Individual and Classroom Differences". Journal for Research in Mathematics Education. 21 (2): 109–122. doi:10.2307/749138. JSTOR 749138.
  2. Elliot, Andrew (2006-06-01). "The Hierarchical Model of Approach-Avoidance Motivation". Motivation and Emotion. 30 (2): 111–116. doi:10.1007/s11031-006-9028-7.
  3. Crouzevillae, Marie. "Performance Goals and Task Performance Integrative Considerations on the Distraction Hypothesis". European Psychologist. 22: 73–82. doi:10.1027/1016-9040/a000281.
  4. Midgley, C. "Performance - Approach Goals: Good for What, For Whom, and Under What Conditions, and at What Cost?". Journal of Educational Psychology. 93: 77–86. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.77.
  5. Bipp, Tanja. "The Effect of Subconscious Performance Goals on Academic Performance". Journal of Experimental Education. 23: 469–485. doi:10.1177/1548051816641874.
  6. Ford, ME.; Nichols, CW. (1987). "A taxonomy of human goals and some possible application". Humans as Self-Constructing Living Systems: Putting the Framework to Work. Hillsdale: Erlbaum. pp. 289–311.
  7. Klahr, David (August 1985). "Solving Problems with Ambiguous Subgoal Ordering: Preschoolers' Performance". Child Development. 56 (4): 940–952. doi:10.2307/1130106. ISSN 0009-3920. JSTOR 1130106.
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.