Ricco's law

Riccò's law, discovered by astronomer Annibale Riccò, is one of several laws that describe a human's ability to visually detect targets on a uniform background.[1] This law explains the visual relationship between a target angular area A and target luminance increment required for detection when that target is unresolved (that is, is too small in the field of view to make out different parts of it).[2] The law is given by:

where is a constant (for a given background, see below).

For constant background luminance , the equation can be restated as

with a different constant . The fraction is referred to as Weber contrast C.

Riccò's law is applicable for regions where the target being detected is unresolved. The resolution of the human eye (the receptive field size) is approximately one arc-minute in the center (the fovea center) but the size increases in peripheral vision. Riccò's law is applicable for targets of angular area less than the size of the receptive field. This region is variable based on the amount of background luminance. Riccò's law is based on the fact that within a receptive field, the light energy (or the number of photons per second) required to lead to the target being detected is summed over the area and is thus proportional to the luminance and to the area.[3] Therefore, the contrast threshold required for detection is proportional to the signal-to-noise ratio multiplied by the noise divided by the area. This leads to the above equation.

The "constant" K is actually a function of the background luminance B to which the eye is assumed to be adapted. It has been shown by Andrew Crumey[4] that for unconstrained vision (that is, observers could either look directly or at the target or avert their gaze) an accurate empirical formula for K is

where c1, c2 are constants taking different values for scotopic and photopic vision. For low B this approximates to the De Vries-Rose Law[5] for threshold contrast C

However, at very low background luminance (less than 10−5 candela per square metre) the threshold value for the illuminance

is a constant (around 10−9 lux) and does not depend on B.[6] In that case

or

At high B such as the daylight sky, Crumey's formula approaches an asymptotic value for K of 5.1×10−9 or 5.4×10−9 lux per nit.[7]

Ricco's Areas

Ricco’s Areas are any areas of the retina where cells can detect a visual stimulus at threshold. The areas will change in location and size depending on the type of cells, the light conditions and the type of stimulus.

gollark: MOSFETs are in fact the most produce electronic device ever. Although that transistor isn't one.
gollark: <@319753218592866315> make Macron and Minoteaur simultaneously.
gollark: Not #1 through #9191837 though.
gollark: Even Minoteaur instance #9191838 is in Nim.
gollark: I see.

See also

  • Spatial summation
  • Weber's law
  • Bloch's law (termporal summation)

References

  1. Riccò A. (1877). Ann. Ottalmol., 6, 373.
  2. Schwartz, Steven H. (2004). Visual Perception: A Clinical Orientation (3 ed.). McGraw-Hill Professional. pp. 46–47. ISBN 0-07-141187-9.
  3. Hood, D. C., & Finkelstein, M. A. (1986). Sensitivity to light. In K. R. Boff, L. Kaufman & J. P. Thomas (Eds.), Handbook of perception and human performance (Vol. I: Sensory processes and perception, pp. 5-1 - 5-66). New York: John Wiley.
  4. Crumey, A. (2014). Human contrast threshold and astronomical visibility. MNRAS 442, 2600–2619.
  5. Rose A. (1948) J. Opt. Soc. Am., 38, 196.
  6. Crumey, op. cit., Sections 1.5, 2.1 and 2.3
  7. From coefficients r4 or from coefficients a3 and a5.
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.