Personality development

Personality development is the relatively enduring pattern of the thoughts, feelings, and behaviors that distinguish individuals from each other.[1] The dominant viewpoint in personality psychology indicates that personality emerges early and continues to develop across one's lifespan.[2] Adult personality traits are believed to have a basis in infant temperament, meaning that individual differences in disposition and behavior appear early in life, potentially before language of conscious self-representation develop.[3] The Five Factor Model of personality maps onto the dimensions of childhood temperament,[4] suggesting that individual differences in levels of the corresponding personality traits (neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness) are present from young ages.[5]


Theories

The development of personality is supported and attempted to be explained by theories of personality.

Psychoanalytic Theory

The Psychoanalytic Theory of personality was developed by Sigmund Freud. This theory consists of three main ideas that make up personality, the id, the ego, and the superego. The three traits control their own sections of the psyche. Personality is developed by the three traits that make up the Psychoanalytic theory conflicting.

Trait Theory

The Trait Theory of personality is one of the main theories in the study of personality. According to this theory, traits make up personality. Traits can be described as patterns of behavior, thought, or emotion.

Social Cognitive Theory

The Social Cognitive Theory focuses on cognitive processes in relation to the development of personality. This theory takes into account how individual's gather information and how they use that information moving forward. The social aspect of this theory is explained by how this theory also views the environment that the individual is in, this includes past experiences. [6]

Evolutionary Theory

The evolutionary theory of personality development is primarily based on the evolutionary process of natural selection.[7]From the evolutionary perspective, evolution resulted in variations of the human mind. Natural selection refined these variations based on their beneficence to humans. Due to human complexity, many opposing personality traits proved to be beneficial in a variety of ways.[8] Primitive humans were collectivists due to tribe culture. The personalities of individuals within a tribe were very similar. The division of labor resulted in differentiation in personality traits in order to achieve a higher efficiency. Differentiation in personality traits increased functionality, therefore becoming adaptive through natural selection. Humans continued to develop personality and individuality through evolution.[9]

Lifespan Theory

Classic theories of personality include Freud's tripartite theory and post-Freudian theory (developmental stage theories and type theories) and indicate that most personality development occurs in childhood, stabilizing by the end of adolescence. Current lifespan perspectives that integrate theory and empirical findings dominate the research literature. The lifespan perspectives of personality are based on the plasticity principle, the principle that personality traits are open systems that can be influenced by the environment at any age.[10] Large-scale longitudinal studies have demonstrated that the most active period of personality development appears to be between the ages of 20-40.[10] Although personality grows increasingly consistent with age and typically plateaus near age 50, personality never reachers a period of total stability.[11][12]

Influencing factors

Personality traits demonstrate moderate levels of continuity, smaller but still significant normative or mean-level changes, and individual differences in change, often late into the life course.[13] This pattern is influenced by genetic, environmental, transactional, and stochastic factors.[14]

Genetics

Twin and adoption studies have demonstrated that the heritability of personality traits ranges from .3-.6, with a mean of .5, indicating that 50% of variation in observable personality traits is attributable to genetic influences.[15] In contrast, family and adoption studies have demonstrated a low heritability factor of .22.[16] A study conducted on German women using an IAT (implicit association test), shows a connection between the function of specific neurotransmitters and the predisposition to have certain personality traits like anxiety or extraversion.[17] With the effects of genetic similarity removed, children from the same family often appear no more alike than randomly selected strangers;[18] yet, identical twins raised apart are nearly as similar in personality as identical twins raised together. These findings suggest is that shared family environment has virtually no effect on personality development, and that similarity between relatives is almost entirely due to shared genetics.[15]

Environmental

The weakness of shared environmental effects in shaping personality surprised many psychologists, spurring research into non-shared environmental effects, the environmental influences that distinguish siblings from one another.[19] The non-shared environment may include differential treatment by parents, individually-distinct reactions to the shared family environment, peer influences, experiences outside the family, and test error in measurement. In adults, the non-shared environment may also include the unique roles and environments experienced after leaving the family of origin.[20] Further effects of environment in adulthood are demonstrated by research suggesting that different work, marital, and family experiences are associated with personality change;[21] these effects are supported by research involving the impact of major positive and negative life events on personality.[22][23]

Gene-environment interactions

A culmination of research suggests that the development of personality occurs in relation to one's genetics, one's environment, and the interaction between one's genetics and environment.[24][25] Van Gestel and Van Broeckhoven (2003) write, “Almost by definition, complex traits originate from interplay between (multiple) genetic factors and environment.”[26] The corresponsive principle of personality development states that “life experiences may accentuate and reinforce the personality characteristics that were partially responsible for the particular environmental elicitations in the first place”.[27] This principle illustrates how gene-environment interactions maintain and reinforce personality throughout the lifespan. Three main types of gene-environment interactions are active (the process by which individuals with certain genotypes select and create environments that facilitate the expression of those genotypes), passive (the process by which genetic parents provide both the genes and the early environmental influences that contribute to the development of a characteristic in their children), and reactive (the process by which non-family individuals respond to the behavior produced by a genotype in characteristic ways).[28][29]

An example of the way environment can moderate the expression of a gene is the finding by Heath, Eaves, and Martin (1998) that marriage was a protective factor against depression in genetically-identical twins, such that the heritability of depression was as low as 29% in a married twin and as high as 51% in an unmarried twin.[30]

Stability of personality

Over the course of an individual's lifespan, the stability of their personality has been shown to be variable, although this variability levels out in adulthood. Behavioral genetics can account for the variability experienced across the lifespan. This is highly evident in the transitions between childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. From childhood to mid-adolescence, the rate of individual differences in personality increases, primarily due to environmental influences. However, genetic influences play a larger role than environmental influences in adulthood, resulting in fewer individual differences in personality between individuals who share similar genetics.[31] The stability of personality across one's lifespan is further evidenced by a longitudinal study conducted on individuals across the span of fifty years from adolescence through adulthood. The results of this longitudinal study suggested that the personality was malleable, although variations in the level of malleability stabilized in adulthood.[32]

A study published in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology examined personality developing in college students based on the Big Five personality trait domains and facets within those domains. The results suggested that the rank-order stabilities of facets were high, with values greater than .50 (indicating a strong correlation); the results for trait domains were similar to individual facets.[33] High rank-order stability is further evidenced by another study that integrated personality structure, process, and development. This study included previous research that indicated high-order rank stability; it also included research that indicated variation in this stability across periods of the lifespan, such as adolescence and adulthood.[34] The stability and variation of personality is explained by a complex interaction between one's genetics and one's environment.[31]

gollark: Maybe it should be aggregated over more time.
gollark: I think this graph is especially great.
gollark: Huh, apparently the IRCd monitor is still operating.
gollark: citrons: yes, if it's electric and does not use "heat pumps" or something.
gollark: I actually also harvest time-series data from ABR and SPUDNET.

References

  1. Caspi A, Roberts BW (2001). "Personality development across the life course: The argument for change and continuity". Psychological Inquiry. 12 (2): 49–66. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli1202_01.
  2. Roberts, B. W., Wood, D., & Caspi, A. (2010). The development of personality traits in adulthood. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervi (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 375-398). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  3. Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Evans DE (2000). "Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (1): 122–135. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.122. PMID 10653510.
  4. Putnam SP, Ellis LK, Rothbart MK (2001). Eliasz A, Angleitner A (eds.). "The structure of temperament from infancy through adolescence". Advances in Research on Temperament. Germany: Pabst Science: 165–182.
  5. Deal JE, Halverson CF, Havill V, Martin R (2005). "Temperament factors as longitudinal predictors of young adult personality". Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 51 (3): 315–334. doi:10.1353/mpq.2005.0015.
  6. "The Social Cognitive Theory". sphweb.bumc.bu.edu. Retrieved 2020-04-30.
  7. Hettema PJ, Deary IJ (11 November 2013). Foundations of Personality. Springer Science & Business Media. p. 7. ISBN 978-94-011-1660-2.
  8. Penke L, Denissen JJ, Miller GF (August 2007). "The Evolution of Personality Variation in Humans and Other Animals" (PDF). European Journal of Personality. 21 (5): 549–587. doi:10.1002/per.629. ISSN 0890-2070.
  9. Bell, M. G. (2010, January 1). Consciousness: The Evolution Of The Self And Personal Individuality. Retrieved November 1, 2014, from www.agenthuman.com/product/evolution_self_personal_individuality.html#selfevol
  10. Roberts, B. W., Wood, D., & Caspi, A. (2010). The development of personality traits in adulthood. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervi (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 375-398). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  11. Roberts BW, Mroczek D (February 2008). "Personality Trait Change in Adulthood". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 17 (1): 31–35. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00543.x. PMC 2743415. PMID 19756219.
  12. Roberts BW, DelVecchio WF (January 2000). "The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: a quantitative review of longitudinal studies". Psychological Bulletin. 126 (1): 3–25. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.3. PMID 10668348.
  13. Roberts BW, Caspi A, Moffitt T (2001). "The kids are alright: Growth and stability in personality development from adolescence to adulthood". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81 (4): 670–683. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.4.670.
  14. Fraley RC, Roberts BW (January 2005). "Patterns of continuity: a dynamic model for conceptualizing the stability of individual differences in psychological constructs across the life course". Psychological Review. 112 (1): 60–74. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.112.1.60. PMID 15631588.
  15. Harkness AR, Lilienfeld SO (1997). "Individual differences science for treatment planning: Personality traits". Psychological Assessment. 9 (4): 349–360. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.9.4.349.
  16. Vukasović, Tena; Bratko, Denis (2015). "Heritability of personality: A meta-analysis of behavior genetic studies". Psychological Bulletin. 141 (4): 769–785. doi:10.1037/bul0000017. ISSN 1939-1455. PMID 25961374.
  17. Osinsky, Roman; Alexander, Nina; Schmitz, Anja; Kuepper, Yvonne; Müller, Eva; Beer, Laura; Koeppe, Lena; Hennig, Jürgen (January 2010). "Genetic Influences on Implicit Measures of Personality". Journal of Individual Differences. 31 (3): 115–123. doi:10.1027/1614-0001/a000007. ISSN 1614-0001.
  18. Turkheimer, E. & Waldron, M. (2000). Non shared environment: A theoretical, methodological, and quantitative review.
  19. Plomin R, Daniels D (June 2011). "Why are children in the same family so different from one another?". International Journal of Epidemiology. 40 (3): 563–82. doi:10.1017/s0140525x00055941. PMC 3147063. PMID 21807642.
  20. Plomin R, Asbury K, Dunn J (April 2001). "Why are children in the same family so different? Nonshared environment a decade later". Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. Revue Canadienne de Psychiatrie. 46 (3): 225–33. doi:10.1177/070674370104600302. PMID 11320676.
  21. Roberts BW, Wood D, Smith JL (2005). "Evaluating Five Factor Theory and social investment perspectives on personality trait development". Journal of Research in Personality. 39: 166–184. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2004.08.002.
  22. Jeronimus BF, Riese H, Sanderman R, Ormel J (October 2014). "Mutual reinforcement between neuroticism and life experiences: a five-wave, 16-year study to test reciprocal causation". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 107 (4): 751–64. doi:10.1037/a0037009. PMID 25111305.
  23. Jeronimus BF, Ormel J, Aleman A, Penninx BW, Riese H (November 2013). "Negative and positive life events are associated with small but lasting change in neuroticism". Psychological Medicine. 43 (11): 2403–15. doi:10.1017/s0033291713000159. PMID 23410535.
  24. Baumert, Anna; Schmitt, Manfred; Perugini, Marco; Johnson, Wendy; Blum, Gabriela; Borkenau, Peter; Costantini, Giulio; Denissen, Jaap J. A.; Fleeson, William; Grafton, Ben; Jayawickreme, Eranda (2017). "Integrating Personality Structure, Personality Process, and Personality Development" (PDF). European Journal of Personality. 31 (5): 503–528. doi:10.1002/per.2115. ISSN 1099-0984.
  25. South SC, Krueger RF (2008). "An interactionist perspective on genetic and environmental contributions to personality". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2 (2): 929–948. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00062.x.
  26. Van Gestel S, Van Broeckhoven C (October 2003). "Genetics of personality: are we making progress?". Molecular Psychiatry. 8 (10): 840–52. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001367. PMID 14515135.
  27. Donnellan, M. B. & Robins, R. W. (2009). The development of personality across the lifespan. In P. J. Corr & G. Matthews (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of personality psychology (pp. 191-204). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  28. Harkness AR, Lilienfeld SO (1997). "Individual differences science for treatment planning: Personality traits". Psychological Assessment. 9 (4): 349–360. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.9.4.349.
  29. Jeronimus BF, Riese H, Sanderman R, Ormel J (October 2014). "Mutual reinforcement between neuroticism and life experiences: a five-wave, 16-year study to test reciprocal causation". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 107 (4): 751–64. doi:10.1037/a0037009. PMID 25111305.
  30. Heath AC, Eaves LJ, Martin NG (September 1998). "Interaction of marital status and genetic risk for symptoms of depression". Twin Research. 1 (3): 119–22. doi:10.1375/136905298320566249. PMID 10051335.
  31. Mõttus R, Briley DA, Zheng A, Mann FD, Engelhardt LE, Tackett JL, et al. (September 2019). "Kids becoming less alike: A behavioral genetic analysis of developmental increases in personality variance from childhood to adolescence". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 117 (3): 635–658. doi:10.1037/pspp0000194. PMC 6687565. PMID 30920282.
  32. Damian, Rodica Ioana; Spengler, Marion; Sutu, Andreea; Roberts, Brent W. (September 2019). "Sixteen going on sixty-six: A longitudinal study of personality stability and change across 50 years". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 117 (3): 674–695. doi:10.1037/pspp0000210. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 30113194.
  33. Klimstra, Theo A.; Noftle, Erik E.; Luyckx, Koen; Goossens, Luc; Robins, Richard W. (August 2018). "Personality development and adjustment in college: A multifaceted, cross-national view" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 115 (2): 338–361. doi:10.1037/pspp0000205. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 30024246.
  34. Baumert, Anna; Schmitt, Manfred; Perugini, Marco; Johnson, Wendy; Blum, Gabriela; Borkenau, Peter; Costantini, Giulio; Denissen, Jaap J. A.; Fleeson, William; Grafton, Ben; Jayawickreme, Eranda (2017). "Integrating Personality Structure, Personality Process, and Personality Development" (PDF). European Journal of Personality. 31 (5): 503–528. doi:10.1002/per.2115. ISSN 1099-0984.
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.