Parallel individuation system

The parallel individuation system, also called object tracking system is a non-symbolic cognitive system that supports the representation of numerical values from zero to three (in infants) or four (in adults and non-human animals). It is one of the two cognitive systems responsible for the representation of number, the other one being the approximate number system.[1] Unlike the approximate number system, which is not precise and provides only an estimation of the number, the parallel individuation system is an exact system and encodes the exact numerical identity of the individual items.[2] The parallel individuation system has been attested in human adults, non-human animals,[2] such as fish[3] and human infants, although performance of infants is dependent on their age and task.[4]

Evidence

The evidence for parallel individuation system comes from a number of experiments on adults, infants and non-human animals. For example, adults perform error-free when they enumerate elements for numerosities from one to four, after which their error rate rises.[4] Similarly, infants of 10 to 12 months represented the values for "exactly one", "exactly two" and "exactly three", but not for higher numbers, in a task based on hidden object retrieval.[4]

Parallel individuation system in animals was demonstrated in an experiment in which guppies were tested on their preference of social groups of different size, under the assumption that they have a preference for bigger size groups. In this experiment, fish successfully discriminated between numbers from 1 to 4 but after this number their performance decreased.[3] However, not all studies find confirmation of this system and for example New Zealand robins showed no difference in their understanding of small (1 to 4) and larger (above 4) amounts.[5]

gollark: The UI bit is going to be password-protected.
gollark: I should really add a function to access websockets to it but meh.
gollark: The shiny new SPUDNET admin UI.
gollark: Yeeeees, and?
gollark: So what?

References

  1. Piazza, M. (2010). "Neurocognitive start-up tools for symbolic number representations". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 14: 542–551. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.008. PMID 21055996.
  2. Hyde, D. (2011). "Two systems of non-symbolic numerical cognition". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 5. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2011.00150. PMC 3228256. PMID 22144955.
  3. Agrillo, Christian (2012). "Evidence for Two Numerical Systems That Are Similar in Humans and Guppies". PLoS ONE. 7 (2): e31923. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0031923. PMC 3280231. PMID 22355405.
  4. Feigenson, L; Dehaene S.; Spelke, E. (2004). "Core systems of number". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 8 (7): 307–314. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.002. PMID 15242690.
  5. Hunt, Simon; Low, Jason; Burns, K. (2008). "Adaptive numerical competency in a food-hoarding songbird". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 275: 2373–2379. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0702. PMC 2603231. PMID 18611847.
This article is issued from Wikipedia. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.