Functional Ensemble of Temperament
Functional Ensemble of Temperament (FET) is a neurochemical model suggesting specific functional roles of main neurotransmitter systems in the regulation of behaviour.
Earlier theories
Medications can adjust the release of brain neurotransmitters in cases of depression, anxiety disorder, schizophrenia and other mental disorders because an imbalance within neurotransmitter systems can emerge as consistent characteristics in behaviour compromising people's lives. All people have a weaker form of such imbalance in at least one of such neurotransmitter systems that make each of us distinct from one another. The impact of this weak imbalance in neurochemistry can be seen in the consistent features of behaviour in healthy people (temperament). In this sense temperament (as neuro-chemically-based individual differences) and mental illness represents varying degrees along the same continuum of neurotransmitter imbalance in neurophysiological systems of behavioural regulation.[1][2][3][4][5][6][7][8][9][10][11][12]
In fact, multiple temperament traits (such as Impulsivity, sensation seeking, neuroticism, endurance, plasticity, sociability or extraversion) have been linked to brain neurotransmitters and hormone systems.[13][14][15][16][17][18][19]
By the end of the 20th century, it became clear that the human brain operates with more than a dozen neurotransmitters and a large number of neuropeptides and hormones. The relationships between these different chemical systems are complex as some of them suppress and some of them induce each other's release during neuronal exchanges. This complexity of relationships devalues the old approach of assigning “inhibitory vs. excitatory” roles to neurotransmitters: the same neurotransmitters can be either inhibitory or excitatory depending on what system they interact with. It became clear that an impressive diversity of neurotransmitters and their receptors is necessary to meet a wide range of behavioural situations, but the links between temperament traits and specific neurotransmitters are still a matter of research. Several attempts were made to assign specific (single) neurotransmitters to specific (single) traits. For example, dopamine was proposed to be a neurotransmitter of the trait of Extraversion, noradrenaline was linked to anxiety, and serotonin was thought to be a neurotransmitter of an inhibition system. These assignments of neurotransmitter functions appeared to be an oversimplification when confronted by the evidence of much more diverse functionality.[17][18] Research groups led by Petra Netter in Germany, Lars Farde in Karolinska Institute in Sweden and Trevor Robbins in Cambridge, UK had most extensive studies of the links between temperament/personality traits or dynamical properties of behavior and groups of neurotransmitters.[14][20][21][22][23][24]
Development of the FET model
The architecture of the Functional Ensemble of Temperament (FET) was developed by Trofimova as the Compact version of the Structure of Temperament Questionnaire (STQ-77) in 1997–2007. This model inherits the activity-specific approach to the structure of temperament proposed by Rusalov in mid-1980s. According to this approach, the traits of temperament (and behavioural regulation) related to motor-physical, social-verbal and mental aspects of activities are based on different neurophysiological systems and should be assessed separately (so you can see a separation of traits into 3 rows related to these 3 types of activities). The final STQ-77/FET model considers 12 systems (and temperament traits): 9 systems (and traits) regulating the formal functional aspects of behaviour (energetic, dynamic and orientational, each assessed in three domains (intellectual, physical and social-verbal) together with 3 systems related to emotionality (Neuroticism, Impulsivity and a disposition of Satisfaction (formerly called Self-Confidence) (see Figure).[25][26][27][28][11]
The differences between Trofimova's and Rusalov's models of temperament (and the structures of their versions of the STQ) are:
- the choice of the grouping of temperament traits by three dynamical aspects (endurance, speed of integration of actions and orientation), presented as three columns in the Figure;
- a presence of orientation-related traits in Trofimova's model which were not included in Rusalov's model. These traits describe the behavioural orientation of a person with preferences to specific types of reinforcers: sensations (Sensation Seeking), other people's state (Empathy) or knowledge about causes of natural processes (a trait named as Sensitivity to Probabilities).
- a different structure of the traits related to emotionality. FET considers emotionality traits as systems amplifying three dynamical aspects of behaviour presented in the three columns of the model. Amplification of orientation aspects emerges in the trait of Neuroticism; amplification of speed of integration (i.e. immature integration) emerges as Impulsivity and amplification of subjective feeling of energetic capacities emerges in the trait of Satisfaction (Self-confidence).
In 2007–2013 this STQ-77 model of temperament was reviewed and compared to the main findings in neurophysiology, neurochemistry, clinical psychology and kinesiology resulting in the neurochemical FET model offered by Irina Trofimova, McMaster University.[17] Trevor Robbins, Cambridge University who collaborated with Trofimova on this project in 2014–2016 suggested a revision of the part of the FET neurochemical hypothesis related to the trait of Intellectual (mental) Endurance (known in cognitive psychology also as "sustained attention"). This neurochemical component of the FET hypothesis was upgraded in 2015 by underlying a key role of acetylcholine and noradrenalin in sustained attention.[17][18][29] In February 2018, by the suggestion of Dr Marina Kolbeneva (Institute of Psychology, Russian Academy of Sciences) the scale Self-Confidence was renamed as the scale of dispositional Satisfaction.
Overview of the model
The final STQ-77/FET model considers 12 systems (and temperament traits): nine systems (and traits) regulating the formal functional aspects of behaviour (energetic, dynamic and orientational) each assessed in three domains (intellectual, physical and social-verbal) together with three systems related to emotionality (Neuroticism, Impulsivity and Satisfaction (Self-Confidence)) (see Figure). The FET hypothesis suggests that the nine non-emotionality traits are regulated by the monoamines (MA) (noradrenalin, dopamine, serotonin), acetylcholine and neuropeptide systems, whereas the three emotionality-related traits emerge as a dysregulation of opioid receptors systems that have direct control over MA systems. Importantly, the FET model suggests that there is no one-to-one correspondence between the neurotransmitter systems underlying temperament traits (or mental disorders) but instead specific ensemble relationships between these systems emerge as temperament traits.[17][18][11] The FET hypothesis is based only on the strongest consensus points in the research studying the role of neurotransmitter in behavioural regulation and the components of temperament; it doesn't list more controversial links between these multiple systems.
Temperament trait | Description | Hypothesized links to neurochemical systems † |
---|---|---|
Behavioral orientation traits | NA +... | |
Sensation seeking | behavioral orientation to well-defined and existing sensational objects and events, underestimation of outcomes of risky behaviour | + cortisol, AdrR, DA, PRL-NPY interactions |
Empathy | behavioral orientation to the emotional states/needs of others (ranging from empathic deafness in autism and schizophrenia disorders to social dependency) | Possible action of OXY, ADH, MOPr interacting with the NA system |
Sensitivity to Probabilities (analytic abilities) | the drive to gather information about commonality, frequency and values of events, to differentiate their specific features, to project these features in future actions | Interaction between neocortical NA, DA, 5-HT and ACh systems |
Speed of action-integration traits | DA +... | |
Physical Tempo | speed of integration of an action in physical manipulations with objects with well-defined scripts of actions | DA-PRL, DA-GABA /Glu interaction in basal ganglia, DOPr |
Social-verbal Tempo | the preferred speed of speech and ability to understand fast speech on well-known topics, reading and sorting of known verbal material | OXY, ADH and PRL under DA control, especially in dorsal striatum |
Plasticity | the ability to adapt quickly to changes in situations, to change the program of action, and to shift between different tasks | DA-5-HT interaction in the cortex-basal ganglia networks |
Maintenance of activity traits | 5-HT, ACh +... | |
Physical Endurance | the ability of an individual to sustain prolonged physical activity using well-defined behavioral elements | 5-HT, ACh, GH-SOM, orexins |
Social-verbal Endurance (sociability) | sociability; the ability of an individual to sustain prolonged social-verbal activities using well-defined behavioral elements | 5-HT, OXY, ADH, GH-PRL, orexins |
Mental Endurance (sustained attention) | the ability to stay focused on selected features of objects with suppression of behavioral reactivity to other features | Neocortical NA-ACh (with a lead of ACh for tonic attention and a lead of NA for novelty aspects) |
Emotional amplifier traits | Opioid receptor systems + ... | |
Neuroticism | A tendency to avoid novelty, unpredictable situations and uncertainty | KOPr > MOPr imbalance →NA-HPA |
Impulsivity | Initiation of actions based on immediate emotional reactivity | DOPr→(DA, MOPr, BDNF, CREB) |
Satisfaction | A sense of security, disposition for a good mood; in high values – overconfidence with negligence to details | KOPr < MOPr →(5-HT, DA), SOM |
† Neurotransmitter systems: 5-HT: serotonin; DA: dopamine; NE: noradrenalin; ACh: acetylcholine; Glu: glutamate; GH: Growth Hormone; SOM: Somatostatin; PRL: prolactin; OXY: oxytocin; ADH: vasopressin; SubP: Substance P; NPY: Neuropeptide Y; KOPr, MOPr, DOPr: kappa-, mu- and delta-opioid receptors correspondingly; AdrR - adrenergic receptors.
References
- Clark, L. A.; Watson, D.; Mineka, S. (1994). "Temperament, personality, and the mood and anxiety disorders". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 103 (1): 103–16. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.103.1.103. PMID 8040472.
- Cloninger CR, ed. (1999). Personality and psychopathology. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Press.
- Mehrabian, Albert (1995). "Distinguishing Depression and Trait Anxiety in Terms of Basic Dimensions of Temperament". Imagination, Cognition and Personality. 15 (2): 133–143. doi:10.2190/JB3J-LL1E-GYGY-D0RJ.
- Ball, S. A.; Tennen, H.; Poling, J. C.; Kranzler, H. R.; Rounsaville, B. J. (1997). "Personality, temperament, and character dimensions and the DSM-IV personality disorders in substance abusers". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 106 (4): 545–53. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.106.4.545. PMID 9358685.
- Brown, Timothy A. (2007). "Temporal course and structural relationships among dimensions of temperament and DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorder constructs". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 116 (2): 313–328. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.116.2.313. PMID 17516764. S2CID 6566116.
- Karam, Elie G.; Salamoun, Mariana M.; Yeretzian, Joumana S.; Mneimneh, Zeina N.; Karam, Aimee N.; Fayyad, John; Hantouche, Elie; Akiskal, Kareen; Akiskal, Hagop S. (2010). "The role of anxious and hyperthymic temperaments in mental disorders: A national epidemiologic study". World Psychiatry. 9 (2): 103–110. doi:10.1002/j.2051-5545.2010.tb00287.x. PMC 2911090. PMID 20671899.
- Watson, David; Naragon-Gainey, Kristin (2014). "Personality, Emotions, and the Emotional Disorders". Clinical Psychological Science. 2 (4): 422–442. doi:10.1177/2167702614536162. PMC 4370336. PMID 25815243.
- Trofimova, IN; Christiansen, J. (2016). "Coupling of temperament traits with mental illness in four age groups". Psychological Reports. 118 (2): 387–412. doi:10.1177/0033294116639430. PMID 27154370.
- Trofimova, IN; Sulis, W (2016). "Benefits of distinguishing between physical and social-verbal aspects of behaviour: an example of generalized anxiety". Frontiers in Psychology. 7: 338. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00338. PMC 4789559. PMID 27014146.
- Trofimova, IN; Sulis, W (2016). "A study of the coupling of FET temperament traits with major depression". Frontiers in Psychology. 7: 1848. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01848. PMC 5123189. PMID 27933018.
- Trofimova, IN (2018). "Functionality vs dimensionality in psychological taxonomies, and a puzzle of emotional valence". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 373, 1744 (1744): 20170167. doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0167. PMC 5832691. PMID 29483351.
- Trofimova, I.; Sulis, W. (2018). "There is more to mental illness than negative affect: Comprehensive temperament profiles in depression and generalized anxiety". BMC Psychiatry. 18 (1): 125. doi:10.1186/s12888-018-1695-x. PMC 5946468. PMID 29747614.
- Gray, J.A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety: an enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- Netter, P. (1991). "Biochemical variables in the study of temperament". In Strelau, J.; Angleitner, A. (eds.). Explorations in temperament: International perspectives on theory and measurement. New York: Plenum Press. pp. 147–161.
- Cloninger, C. Robert; Svrakic, D. M.; Przybeck, T. R. (1993). "A Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character". Archives of General Psychiatry. 50 (12): 975–90. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820240059008. PMID 8250684.
- Depue, Richard A.; Morrone-Strupinsky, Jeannine V. (2005). "A neurobehavioral model of affiliative bonding: Implications for conceptualizing a human trait of affiliation". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 28 (3): 313–50, discussion 350–95. doi:10.1017/S0140525X05000063. PMID 16209725. S2CID 23949957.
- Trofimova, IN (2016). "The interlocking between functional aspects of activities and a neurochemical model of adult temperament". In Arnold, M.C. (ed.). Temperaments: Individual Differences, Social and Environmental Influences and Impact on Quality of Life. New York: Nova Science Publishers. pp. 77–147.
- Trofimova, IN; Robbins, TW (2016). "Temperament and arousal systems: a new synthesis of differential psychology and functional neurochemistry". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 64: 382–402. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.008. PMID 26969100.
- Depue, R.; Fu, Y. (2012). "Neurobiology and neurochemistry of temperament in adults". In Zentner, M.; Shiner, R. (eds.). Handbook of Temperament. NY: Guilford Publications. pp. 368–399.
- Netter, P. (2006). "Dopamine challenge tests as an indicator of psychological traits". Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental. 21 (2): 91–99. doi:10.1002/hup.754. PMID 16444772.
- Netter, P.; Hennig, J.; Roed, I.S. (1996). "Serotonin and Dopamine as Mediators of Sensation Seeking Behavior". Neuropsychobiology. 34 (3): 155–165. doi:10.1159/000119318. PMID 8916073. S2CID 3216894.
- Robbins, T.; Roberts, A. (2007). "Differential Regulation of Fronto-Executive Function by the Monoamines and Acetylcholine". Cerebral Cortex. 17: i151–i160. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm066. PMID 17725997. S2CID 2689998.
- Walker, S. C.; Robbins, T. W.; Roberts, A. C. (2009). "Differential Contributions of Dopamine and Serotonin to Orbitofrontal Cortex Function in the Marmoset". Cerebral Cortex. 19 (4): 889–898. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn136. PMC 2651474. PMID 18723695.
- Robbins, T.W.; Everitt, B.J. (1996). "Arousal Systems and Attention". In Gazzaniga, M. (ed.). The Cognitive Neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. pp. 703–720.
- Rusalov, VM; Trofimova, IN (2007). Structure of Temperament and Its Measurement. Toronto, Canada: Psychological Services Press.
- Trofimova, IN (2010). "Questioning the "general arousal" models". Open Behavioral Science and Psychology. 4: 1–8. doi:10.2174/1874230001004010001.
- Trofimova, IN (2010). "An investigation into differences between the structure of temperament and the structure of personality". American Journal of Psychology. 123 (4): 467–480. doi:10.5406/amerjpsyc.123.4.0467. S2CID 35517343.
- Trofimova, IN; Sulis, W (2011). "Is temperament activity-specific? Validation of the Structure of Temperament Questionnaire – Compact (STQ-77)". International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy. 11 (3): 389–400.
- Robbins, T. W. (2018). "Opinion on monoaminergic contributions to traits and temperament". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 373 (1744): 20170153. doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0153. PMC 5832679. PMID 29483339.