Scientific objectivity

Scientific objectivity is an ideal goal that scientists strive to achieve. Unfortunately, because science is a human enterprise, complete objectivity can never be attained. However, the scientific method is designed to safeguard against bias as much as is possible.

Eyes wearing
inverted lenses

Philosophy of science
Foundations
Method
Conclusions
v - t - e

Truth to nature and the origins of scientific objectivity

The modern concept of scientific objectivity didn't come into existence until the 19th century. The concept of "truth to nature," employed in early scientific atlases, was the forerunner of scientific objectivity. The idea behind truth to nature was to present an idealized version of the object under investigation, which was often done using composite drawings. A large interpretive (i.e., subjective) element was thus inherent in this process. During the mid-19th century, realistic depictions came to be valued over idealized ones and objectivity came to be seen as a moral virtue, giving a new meaning to truth to nature. Individual objects were depicted in atlases instead of composite drawings. The invention of photography during this period allowed for easier standardization of pictures in the atlases. Interpretation and value judgments were de-emphasized as the modern concept of scientific objectivity emerged.[1]

Sources of bias

Methods of limiting bias

There are a number of methods used to limit bias in the course of carrying out an experiment or scientific investigation. What these methods are will vary by discipline, but there are some common ways this is accomplished.

Challenges to the concept of objective and value-free science

  • Thomas Kuhn's concept of paradigm shifts.
  • Marxist critiques of "capitalist science," especially by the radical science movement of the 1960s-70s.[3]
  • Helen Longino's argument that values will tend to affect the choice of a theory when it is underdetermined by data.[4]
  • The degree to which perception and observation is theory-laden as demonstrated by cognitive science.[5]
  • Postmodernism
gollark: Basically, it uses Bucklescript (OCaml -> JS) and Rollup (JS -> slightly different JS) and getting them to cooperate is horrible, and also everything else is aargh.
gollark: I've been configuring a project which relies on JS build systems.
gollark: Otter madness. Otter, otter madness.
gollark: Oh, cool, the full database. At last I can start Markov-chaining even more chat messages.
gollark: Which I expected to not be too problematic, as it wasn't rebooted much. They were banned when chervil spammed using my relay thing.

See also

References

  1. Lorraine Daston and Peter Galison. The Image of Objectivity. Representations, vol. 0, no. 40 (special issue), 1992, pp. 81-128
  2. Daniele Fanelli. Do Pressures to Publish Increase Scientists' Bias? An Empirical Support from US States Data. PLoS ONE 5(4): e10271.
  3. Gary Werskey. The Marxist Critique of Capitalist Science: A History in Three Movements? Science as Culture
  4. Helen Longino. Beyond "Bad Science": Skeptical Reflections on the Value-Freedom of Scientific Inquiry. Science, Technology, & Human Values, Vol. 8, No. 1 (Winter, 1983), pp. 7-17
  5. William F. Brewer and Bruce L. Lambert. The Theory-Ladenness of Observation and the Theory-Ladenness of the Rest of the Scientific Process. Philosophy of Science Vol. 68, No. 3, 2001, pp. S176-S186
This article is issued from Rationalwiki. The text is licensed under Creative Commons - Attribution - Sharealike. Additional terms may apply for the media files.